Can highly arousing traumatic Experiences be repressed?

IF 2.2 2区 社会学 Q1 CRIMINOLOGY & PENOLOGY
Israel Nachson
{"title":"Can highly arousing traumatic Experiences be repressed?","authors":"Israel Nachson","doi":"10.1111/lcrp.3_12273","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Reading Mazzoni et al.'s paper, I was glad to find out that their and my own paper (Nachson, <span>2025</span>) are in agreement with each other in terms of both, the issues involved in the concepts of repression and of recovered memory, and their solutions. Although our conceptual perspectives differ from each other—as evident by the differential bibliography lists—the similarity between our analyses is indeed striking, so as to render them complementary.</p><p>We similarly delineate the difficulties involved in the very definition of the concept of repression which is not operationally defined, and hence cannot be scientifically validated. Thus, while ‘soft-minded’ clinicians find the concept useful for treating clients who have allegedly recovered memories of childhood sexual abuses, ‘hard-minded’ researchers find it misleading.</p><p>The latter also shy away from the notion that a given behaviour may be considered an exception to a rule. Therefore, the special properties assigned to memories of traumatic events (such as resistance to distortion over time) cannot be entertained without solid evidence, and indeed, cognitive scientists tend to reject the notion of a special nature of traumatic memories.</p><p>The insistence on a special nature reminds one of other failing attempts to postulate an explanation of a given behaviour in terms of exceptional processes; for example, the original, ‘frustration-aggression hypothesis’ (Dollard et al., <span>1939</span>), which was based on the notion that, unlike other behavioural responses which are learned, aggression is exceptionally caused by a single factor, namely frustration.</p><p>Mazzoni et al. make it clear that evidence showing that memory of traumatic experiences is less accessible and less retrievable than that of non-traumatic experiences does not prove that it is repressed, and in-fact, it might even be enhanced rather than impaired. The studies showing detrimental effects of trauma on memory in animals, and those performed on intentional suppression, do not constitute, according to the authors, evidence for repression in humans.</p><p>The authors' conclusion that each of the two opposing opinions regarding recovered memories entails a partial truth reminds one of the American Psychological Association's decision to accept both of them as valid (Howard &amp; Tuffin, <span>2002</span>), and of my discussion on multiple truths (Nachson, <span>2025</span>).</p><p>As a solution of the on-going debate regarding recovered memories, the authors suggest a ‘middle ground hypothesis’, according to which only traumatic memories of highly arousing, negative experiences might be repressed. However, this suggestion begs the question, since the debate focuses on memories of sexual abuses which are clearly ‘highly arousing, negative experiences’; so we are back on square one.</p><p>Finally, I wholeheartedly agree with the authors that further research on recovery of memory is called for, and that the clinical and legal communities should be fully informed about the issues involved with the theory and application of the concept of recovered memories, so as to avoid their uncritical acceptance.</p>","PeriodicalId":18022,"journal":{"name":"Legal and Criminological Psychology","volume":"30 S1","pages":"107-108"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2000,"publicationDate":"2025-03-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lcrp.3_12273","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Legal and Criminological Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lcrp.3_12273","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"CRIMINOLOGY & PENOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Reading Mazzoni et al.'s paper, I was glad to find out that their and my own paper (Nachson, 2025) are in agreement with each other in terms of both, the issues involved in the concepts of repression and of recovered memory, and their solutions. Although our conceptual perspectives differ from each other—as evident by the differential bibliography lists—the similarity between our analyses is indeed striking, so as to render them complementary.

We similarly delineate the difficulties involved in the very definition of the concept of repression which is not operationally defined, and hence cannot be scientifically validated. Thus, while ‘soft-minded’ clinicians find the concept useful for treating clients who have allegedly recovered memories of childhood sexual abuses, ‘hard-minded’ researchers find it misleading.

The latter also shy away from the notion that a given behaviour may be considered an exception to a rule. Therefore, the special properties assigned to memories of traumatic events (such as resistance to distortion over time) cannot be entertained without solid evidence, and indeed, cognitive scientists tend to reject the notion of a special nature of traumatic memories.

The insistence on a special nature reminds one of other failing attempts to postulate an explanation of a given behaviour in terms of exceptional processes; for example, the original, ‘frustration-aggression hypothesis’ (Dollard et al., 1939), which was based on the notion that, unlike other behavioural responses which are learned, aggression is exceptionally caused by a single factor, namely frustration.

Mazzoni et al. make it clear that evidence showing that memory of traumatic experiences is less accessible and less retrievable than that of non-traumatic experiences does not prove that it is repressed, and in-fact, it might even be enhanced rather than impaired. The studies showing detrimental effects of trauma on memory in animals, and those performed on intentional suppression, do not constitute, according to the authors, evidence for repression in humans.

The authors' conclusion that each of the two opposing opinions regarding recovered memories entails a partial truth reminds one of the American Psychological Association's decision to accept both of them as valid (Howard & Tuffin, 2002), and of my discussion on multiple truths (Nachson, 2025).

As a solution of the on-going debate regarding recovered memories, the authors suggest a ‘middle ground hypothesis’, according to which only traumatic memories of highly arousing, negative experiences might be repressed. However, this suggestion begs the question, since the debate focuses on memories of sexual abuses which are clearly ‘highly arousing, negative experiences’; so we are back on square one.

Finally, I wholeheartedly agree with the authors that further research on recovery of memory is called for, and that the clinical and legal communities should be fully informed about the issues involved with the theory and application of the concept of recovered memories, so as to avoid their uncritical acceptance.

高度唤起的创伤经历能被压抑吗?
在阅读Mazzoni等人的论文时,我很高兴地发现,他们和我自己的论文(Nachson, 2025)在压抑和恢复记忆概念所涉及的问题以及它们的解决方案方面都是一致的。虽然我们的概念观点彼此不同——正如不同的参考书目列表所表明的那样——但我们的分析之间的相似性确实是惊人的,从而使它们互补。我们同样描述了在镇压概念的定义中所涉及的困难,因为镇压概念在操作上没有定义,因此无法科学地验证。因此,虽然“软心肠”的临床医生发现这个概念对治疗那些据称恢复了童年性虐待记忆的客户很有用,但“硬心肠”的研究人员发现它具有误导性。后者也回避将特定行为视为规则例外的概念。因此,没有确凿的证据,创伤性事件记忆的特殊属性(比如随着时间的推移对扭曲的抵抗力)是不能被接受的,事实上,认知科学家倾向于拒绝创伤性事件记忆具有特殊性质的概念。对特殊性质的坚持让人想起了另一种失败的尝试,即用特殊过程来假设对特定行为的解释;例如,最初的“挫折-攻击假说”(Dollard et al., 1939),它基于这样一种观念:与其他习得的行为反应不同,攻击通常是由单一因素引起的,即挫折。Mazzoni等人明确指出,有证据表明创伤性经历的记忆比非创伤性经历的记忆更难以获得和检索,但这并不能证明创伤性经历的记忆被压抑了,事实上,创伤性经历的记忆甚至可能被增强而不是受损。这些研究表明,创伤对动物记忆的有害影响,以及那些故意压抑的研究,并不构成人类压抑的证据。作者的结论是,关于恢复记忆的两种对立观点中的每一种都包含部分事实,这让人想起了美国心理协会决定接受这两种观点都是有效的(Howard &;Tuffin, 2002),以及我对多重真理的讨论(Nachson, 2025)。为了解决关于恢复记忆的持续争论,作者提出了一个“中间假设”,根据该假设,只有高度唤起的负面经历的创伤性记忆可能会被抑制。然而,这个建议回避了一个问题,因为辩论的重点是性虐待的记忆,这显然是“高度唤起的负面经历”;所以我们又回到了起点。最后,我完全同意作者的观点,即需要对记忆恢复进行进一步的研究,临床和法律界应该充分了解与恢复记忆概念的理论和应用有关的问题,以避免他们不加批判地接受。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
4.00
自引率
4.30%
发文量
31
期刊介绍: Legal and Criminological Psychology publishes original papers in all areas of psychology and law: - victimology - policing and crime detection - crime prevention - management of offenders - mental health and the law - public attitudes to law - role of the expert witness - impact of law on behaviour - interviewing and eyewitness testimony - jury decision making - deception The journal publishes papers which advance professional and scientific knowledge defined broadly as the application of psychology to law and interdisciplinary enquiry in legal and psychological fields.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信