Reflections on British False Memory Society cases, middle ground, and inferring internal mental processes

IF 2.2 2区 社会学 Q1 CRIMINOLOGY & PENOLOGY
Lawrence Patihis, Kevin Felstead
{"title":"Reflections on British False Memory Society cases, middle ground, and inferring internal mental processes","authors":"Lawrence Patihis,&nbsp;Kevin Felstead","doi":"10.1111/lcrp.3_12274","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Nachson's commentary (<span>2025</span>) reveals that although Israel has some differences in dealing with recovered memory cases, there are some similar concerns. On the one hand, Israel deals with such cases with professional judges, and not juries as is usually done in the UK. Nachson notes that these professional judges can be affected by the emotional testimony of the accusers, as opposed to the less emotional accused. This may be a similar dynamic in the UK, but just with jurors. Nachson also relays the fascinating example from an Isreali court case in which an expert witness for the prosecution elevated the memory evidence to such a degree that it trumped other considerations. As we reflect on our data in light of Nachson's comments, it occurs to us that we should point out that out of the total 2364 cases shown in our Table 1 (Patihis &amp; Felstead, <span>2025</span>) only 227 were found guilty in a court of law, which is 10% of cases. In other words, the legal system may work well in most cases, though the number found guilty out of those in which police pursue a case was 27%. We appreciate Nachson's (<span>2025</span>) concurrence on objective truth in cases, which clarifies his meaning Nachson (<span>2025-a</span>).</p><p>Krackow et al.'s (<span>2025</span>) commentary mentions that to reduce wrongful convictions as false memory stories go out of fashion in the media, that regular reminders of the problem be communicated to the public. This is a good idea that could be extended to legal professionals as well. Their second suggestion of false memory societies recording eating disorders, self-harm, and therapy use, are excellent and insightful of the qualitative pattern we have seen in the cases in the caseload files. Almost all cases involved therapy, though we did not quantify that in our data. Krackow et al.'s final suggestion is perhaps the unarticulated conclusion of our data: that practitioners should avoid using memory recovery or enhancement techniques.</p><p>To round out our commentaries, we return to a central point we made in the discussion of Patihis and Felstead (<span>2025</span>). The idea of a middle ground can be useful in the sense that a group of well trained scientists might hash out questions of prevalence of a problem in society, size of effects, point out each other's confirmation biases, and so on. Nevertheless, the idea of a middle ground can be a little relativistic about whether there is an objective truth to approximate to.</p><p>In this regards, we think those promoting dissociative amnesia as a mental process are not being scientific, and that is as true as Freud in the 1890s, or an esteemed psychiatrist today at an elite university. A solid approach to science involves great caution in assuming invisible internal mental processes that are a step too far beyond a measurable behavior. The behaviorists were fruitful in scientific discovery, and the cognitive psychologists subsequently were admirably cautious in inferring internal mental processes, and also achieved tremendous scientific fruitfulness. Cognitive process that are proximal to a measurable behavior are defensible, but not processes that infer too much. In this regard, assuming memory processes (such as storage, retrieval, consolidation, errors, etc) from behavioral responses is in the realm of science, but the concept of dissociation or dissociative amnesia are just inferring too much from the observed behaviours. Memory-report behaviors (verbal, written, keypress, etc) or neural activation in a brain area, are just not proximal enough to the very complex proposed invisible processes such as dissociation or dissociative amnesia, and these processes cannot be inferred from such data.</p>","PeriodicalId":18022,"journal":{"name":"Legal and Criminological Psychology","volume":"30 S1","pages":"74-75"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2000,"publicationDate":"2025-03-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lcrp.3_12274","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Legal and Criminological Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/lcrp.3_12274","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"社会学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"CRIMINOLOGY & PENOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Nachson's commentary (2025) reveals that although Israel has some differences in dealing with recovered memory cases, there are some similar concerns. On the one hand, Israel deals with such cases with professional judges, and not juries as is usually done in the UK. Nachson notes that these professional judges can be affected by the emotional testimony of the accusers, as opposed to the less emotional accused. This may be a similar dynamic in the UK, but just with jurors. Nachson also relays the fascinating example from an Isreali court case in which an expert witness for the prosecution elevated the memory evidence to such a degree that it trumped other considerations. As we reflect on our data in light of Nachson's comments, it occurs to us that we should point out that out of the total 2364 cases shown in our Table 1 (Patihis & Felstead, 2025) only 227 were found guilty in a court of law, which is 10% of cases. In other words, the legal system may work well in most cases, though the number found guilty out of those in which police pursue a case was 27%. We appreciate Nachson's (2025) concurrence on objective truth in cases, which clarifies his meaning Nachson (2025-a).

Krackow et al.'s (2025) commentary mentions that to reduce wrongful convictions as false memory stories go out of fashion in the media, that regular reminders of the problem be communicated to the public. This is a good idea that could be extended to legal professionals as well. Their second suggestion of false memory societies recording eating disorders, self-harm, and therapy use, are excellent and insightful of the qualitative pattern we have seen in the cases in the caseload files. Almost all cases involved therapy, though we did not quantify that in our data. Krackow et al.'s final suggestion is perhaps the unarticulated conclusion of our data: that practitioners should avoid using memory recovery or enhancement techniques.

To round out our commentaries, we return to a central point we made in the discussion of Patihis and Felstead (2025). The idea of a middle ground can be useful in the sense that a group of well trained scientists might hash out questions of prevalence of a problem in society, size of effects, point out each other's confirmation biases, and so on. Nevertheless, the idea of a middle ground can be a little relativistic about whether there is an objective truth to approximate to.

In this regards, we think those promoting dissociative amnesia as a mental process are not being scientific, and that is as true as Freud in the 1890s, or an esteemed psychiatrist today at an elite university. A solid approach to science involves great caution in assuming invisible internal mental processes that are a step too far beyond a measurable behavior. The behaviorists were fruitful in scientific discovery, and the cognitive psychologists subsequently were admirably cautious in inferring internal mental processes, and also achieved tremendous scientific fruitfulness. Cognitive process that are proximal to a measurable behavior are defensible, but not processes that infer too much. In this regard, assuming memory processes (such as storage, retrieval, consolidation, errors, etc) from behavioral responses is in the realm of science, but the concept of dissociation or dissociative amnesia are just inferring too much from the observed behaviours. Memory-report behaviors (verbal, written, keypress, etc) or neural activation in a brain area, are just not proximal enough to the very complex proposed invisible processes such as dissociation or dissociative amnesia, and these processes cannot be inferred from such data.

对英国假记忆协会案例、中间立场和推断内部心理过程的思考
Nachson的评论(2025)显示,尽管以色列在处理恢复记忆案例方面存在一些差异,但也存在一些相似的担忧。一方面,以色列由专业法官处理此类案件,而不是像英国那样通常由陪审团处理。Nachson指出,这些专业法官可能会受到原告情绪化证词的影响,而不是受到不那么情绪化的被告的影响。英国可能也有类似的情况,但只是针对陪审员。纳克森还讲述了一个以色列法庭案件的有趣例子,在这个案件中,控方的一位专家证人将记忆证据提升到如此高的程度,以至于它胜过了其他考虑因素。当我们根据Nachson的评论反思我们的数据时,我们想到我们应该指出,在我们表1中显示的总共2364个案例中(Patihis &;Felstead, 2025)只有227人在法庭上被判有罪,占案件的10%。换句话说,法律体系在大多数情况下可能运作良好,尽管在警方追查的案件中,被判有罪的人数占27%。我们赞赏Nachson(2025)对案件客观真理的认同,这澄清了他的意思Nachson (2025-a)。Krackow等人(2025)的评论提到,随着虚假记忆故事在媒体中过时,为了减少错误的定罪,需要定期向公众传达有关该问题的提醒。这是一个很好的想法,也可以扩展到法律专业人士。他们提出的第二个错误记忆社会记录了饮食失调,自我伤害和治疗的使用,这是我们在案例文件中看到的定性模式的优秀和深刻的见解。几乎所有的病例都涉及治疗,尽管我们没有在我们的数据中量化。Krackow等人最后的建议可能是我们的数据未明确的结论:从业者应该避免使用记忆恢复或增强技术。为了完善我们的评论,我们回到我们在讨论帕蒂斯和费尔斯特德(2025)时提出的一个中心点。在某种意义上,中间立场的想法是有用的,一群训练有素的科学家可能会讨论一个问题在社会中的普遍性、影响的大小、指出彼此的确认偏差等等。然而,关于是否存在一个可以近似的客观真理,中间立场的想法可能有点相对主义。在这方面,我们认为那些将分离性健忘症作为一种心理过程的人是不科学的,这就像19世纪90年代的弗洛伊德,或者今天精英大学里一位受人尊敬的精神病学家一样正确。一个可靠的科学方法涉及到非常谨慎地假设不可见的内在心理过程,这些过程远远超出了可测量的行为。行为主义者在科学发现方面硕果累累,认知心理学家随后在推断内部心理过程方面非常谨慎,也取得了巨大的科学成果。接近可测量行为的认知过程是站得住脚的,但过度推断的过程则不然。在这方面,假设行为反应的记忆过程(如存储,检索,巩固,错误等)属于科学领域,但解离或解离性健忘症的概念只是从观察到的行为中进行了过多的推断。记忆报告行为(口头,书面,按键等)或大脑区域的神经激活,只是不足以接近非常复杂的无形过程,如分离或分离性健忘症,这些过程不能从这些数据中推断出来。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
4.00
自引率
4.30%
发文量
31
期刊介绍: Legal and Criminological Psychology publishes original papers in all areas of psychology and law: - victimology - policing and crime detection - crime prevention - management of offenders - mental health and the law - public attitudes to law - role of the expert witness - impact of law on behaviour - interviewing and eyewitness testimony - jury decision making - deception The journal publishes papers which advance professional and scientific knowledge defined broadly as the application of psychology to law and interdisciplinary enquiry in legal and psychological fields.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信