Behavioural mimicry among poison frogs diverges during close-range encounters with predators.

IF 2.1 3区 生物学 Q3 ECOLOGY
James B Barnett, Brendan L McEwen, Isaac Kinley, Hannah M Anderson, Justin Yeager
{"title":"Behavioural mimicry among poison frogs diverges during close-range encounters with predators.","authors":"James B Barnett, Brendan L McEwen, Isaac Kinley, Hannah M Anderson, Justin Yeager","doi":"10.1093/jeb/voaf038","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Aposematic species signal their unpalatability to potential predators with recognisable, and frequently conspicuous, colour patterns. These visual signals are often also associated with bold behaviour and a reduced propensity to escape from approaching predators. Bold behaviours may act as an aversive signal and allow defended prey to avoid the energetic/opportunity costs that arise from fleeing predators. For Batesian mimics, non-defended species which replicate the colours of defended models, behavioural mimicry may also improve mimic fidelity and reduce energetic/opportunity costs. However, as predators may test the honesty of aposematic signals through sampling behaviour, Batesian mimics can be at high risk during close-range interactions with predators. This raises the question of whether/when Batesian mimics should deviate from behavioural mimicry and initiate more extensive escape behaviour. Here, we exposed the chemically defended poison frog Ameerega bilinguis and its (non-toxic) Batesian mimic Allobates zaparo to a simulated predator encounter. We predicted Al. zaparo would escape to a greater distance and in a more erratic manner than Am. bilinguis. Yet, contrary to our predictions, Al. zaparo did not flee far from predators. It was, however, more likely to initiate escape prior to physical contact from the predator. We suggest that bold behaviour coupled with pre-emptive movement allows Al. zaparo to retain the benefits of behavioural mimicry while reducing the likelihood that predators will test signal honesty. Our data highlight, that when examining the evolution of mimicry, we must consider both morphological and behavioural traits, as well as how risk to the prey may change how they behave throughout the predation sequence.</p>","PeriodicalId":50198,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Evolutionary Biology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1000,"publicationDate":"2025-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Evolutionary Biology","FirstCategoryId":"99","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jeb/voaf038","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"ECOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Aposematic species signal their unpalatability to potential predators with recognisable, and frequently conspicuous, colour patterns. These visual signals are often also associated with bold behaviour and a reduced propensity to escape from approaching predators. Bold behaviours may act as an aversive signal and allow defended prey to avoid the energetic/opportunity costs that arise from fleeing predators. For Batesian mimics, non-defended species which replicate the colours of defended models, behavioural mimicry may also improve mimic fidelity and reduce energetic/opportunity costs. However, as predators may test the honesty of aposematic signals through sampling behaviour, Batesian mimics can be at high risk during close-range interactions with predators. This raises the question of whether/when Batesian mimics should deviate from behavioural mimicry and initiate more extensive escape behaviour. Here, we exposed the chemically defended poison frog Ameerega bilinguis and its (non-toxic) Batesian mimic Allobates zaparo to a simulated predator encounter. We predicted Al. zaparo would escape to a greater distance and in a more erratic manner than Am. bilinguis. Yet, contrary to our predictions, Al. zaparo did not flee far from predators. It was, however, more likely to initiate escape prior to physical contact from the predator. We suggest that bold behaviour coupled with pre-emptive movement allows Al. zaparo to retain the benefits of behavioural mimicry while reducing the likelihood that predators will test signal honesty. Our data highlight, that when examining the evolution of mimicry, we must consider both morphological and behavioural traits, as well as how risk to the prey may change how they behave throughout the predation sequence.

在与捕食者的近距离接触中,毒蛙的行为模仿出现了分歧。
示警的物种用可识别的、经常很显眼的颜色图案向潜在的捕食者表明它们不好吃。这些视觉信号通常也与大胆的行为和减少逃避接近的捕食者的倾向有关。大胆的行为可能是一种厌恶的信号,让被防御的猎物避免因逃离捕食者而产生的能量/机会成本。对于贝叶斯模仿者(复制被防御模型的颜色的非防御物种)来说,行为模仿也可以提高模仿保真度并降低能量/机会成本。然而,由于捕食者可能会通过采样行为来测试警告信号的真实性,贝叶斯模拟在与捕食者的近距离互动中可能会面临高风险。这就提出了一个问题,即贝叶斯模仿者是否/何时应该偏离行为模仿者,并启动更广泛的逃避行为。在这里,我们将化学防御的毒蛙Ameerega bilinguis和它的(无毒的)贝叶斯模拟物Allobates zaparo暴露在模拟的捕食者遭遇中。我们预测al - zaparo会以比Am更不稳定的方式逃离到更远的地方。bilinguis。然而,与我们的预测相反,al - zaparo并没有逃离捕食者。然而,在与捕食者进行身体接触之前,它更有可能主动逃跑。我们认为,大胆的行为加上先发制人的行动使al - zaparo保留了行为模仿的好处,同时减少了捕食者测试信号诚实的可能性。我们的数据强调,当研究模仿的进化时,我们必须考虑形态和行为特征,以及猎物的风险如何改变它们在整个捕食序列中的行为。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Journal of Evolutionary Biology
Journal of Evolutionary Biology 生物-进化生物学
CiteScore
4.20
自引率
4.80%
发文量
152
审稿时长
3-6 weeks
期刊介绍: It covers both micro- and macro-evolution of all types of organisms. The aim of the Journal is to integrate perspectives across molecular and microbial evolution, behaviour, genetics, ecology, life histories, development, palaeontology, systematics and morphology.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信