Editorial: An autism case series, vaccine hesitancy, and death by measles

IF 6.5 1区 医学 Q1 PSYCHIATRY
Eric Fombonne
{"title":"Editorial: An autism case series, vaccine hesitancy, and death by measles","authors":"Eric Fombonne","doi":"10.1111/jcpp.14058","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Measles are back. Larger and more frequent measles outbreaks have been reported in 2024 in the United Kingdom and the United States, which predated the COVID-19 pandemic. Measles deaths worldwide rose to an estimated 136,000 in 2022, mostly children.</p><p>Immunizations have been one of the major contributor to the 20th century increased life expectancy alongside better nutrition, hygiene, and lifestyle, way ahead of medical technological prowess that keeps impressing us. Vaccination campaigns and other preventive policies (e.g. car safety belts and anti-tobacco campaigns) have saved and continue to save lives and reduce morbidity (remember tuberculosis or poliomyelitis) much more than da Vinci surgical robots or heart transplants. Vaccines are safe and post-licensure vaccine safety is continuously monitored with different overlapping population-based surveillance systems (Buttery &amp; Clothier, <span>2022</span>). Vaccines achieved the total eradication of smallpox in the late 1970s. A worldwide campaign to eradicate measles was well under way in the 1990s. Then, a case series published in a prestigious medical journal triggered fears of vaccine-induced autism in the public. Despite rigorous science rapidly dismissing the original claim, the measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccine scare and fear of autism propagated. Years later, it has morphed into what we now refer to as ‘vaccine hesitancy’ (VH), MMR vaccine uptake is still below optimal levels, fueling ongoing measles outbreaks. How did we get there?</p><p>The saga started in 1998 with an editorial decision by the <i>Lancet</i> to publish a case series of 12 children suggesting a new syndrome of autism triggered by MMR vaccination had been discovered (Wakefield et al., <span>1998</span>). The publication of a case series in the <i>Lancet</i> almost seems a contradiction in terms. As researchers and editors, we know how cautious we must remain before drawing causal inferences between two variables. There is an established hierarchy of research designs based on their relative strengths for causality assessment. Experimental designs are at the top (e.g. the RCT), followed by controlled observational (cohort and case–control) studies, and by much weaker ecological studies (relying on confounded correlations between group-level data); at the very bottom, lies the case series which, at most, helps generate hypotheses (especially when the knowledge base is quasi-inexistant) but is never sufficient to test causal ones.</p><p>While it is fair to assume that those who reviewed the 1998 manuscript and those who made the decision to publish it were unaware at the time of the fraudulent nature of the data (see: Godlee, Smith, and Marcovitch (<span>2011</span>); Deer <span>2011a</span>, <span>2011b</span> and Deer (<span>2020</span>)), multiple red flags were readily noticeable: its first author had spent previous years trying to prove that adult inflammatory bowel disorders were linked to measles vaccine, in studies that were unreplicated and dismissed by academic peers; having 12 children with autism ‘referred’ to an adult gastro-enterological surgeon was suspicious; no co-author had autism expertise; there was no control group of any sort; the MMR-autism connection entirely relied on unverified retrospective parental accounts asserting onset of autism <i>days</i> after MMR immunization in a previously normally developing child; regressive autism was presented as a novel syndrome (some Kanner's cases experienced developmental regression and loss of skills); implausible persistence of the measles virus in gut tissues. These accounts were unreliable and insufficient to speculate about a novel etiology of autism.</p><p>Despite numerous failures in the integrity of the peer review and editorial process (Deer, <span>2020</span>), the predicted high impact of this article influenced the <i>Lancet</i> decision to publish. At JCPP, too, editors consider the potential impact of submitted articles as a criterion favoring acceptance for publication based on estimating the resulting increment in scientific knowledge. Instead, Wakefield et al. (<span>1998</span>)'s impact consisted of sensationalistic journalism, greatness of its author and institutional prestige, with disregard for the predictable negative public health consequences. By 2004–2005, the scientific community had conducted several independent population-based controlled studies that all failed to support a link between MMR vaccine and autism (I summarized them in Plotkin et al., <span>2020</span>). Anecdotally, in one of these studies, we (with colleagues at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine) completed a large case–control study in England on the putative links between MMR vaccine and autism. It takes years to complete such studies and when in 2003 we submitted our manuscript to the <i>Lancet</i>, the journal (with the same editor) first returned it without review (‘it is no longer an interesting hypothesis’!). Our study had compared 1,294 autism cases to 4,469 matched controls and showed no association. This negative study was significant for public health but it was unworthy of media coverage. It took a persuasive phone call from one of us to challenge the initial rejection, have the manuscript sent out for review and finally published (Smeeth et al., <span>2004</span>). The contrast between the editorial paths of these two manuscripts is striking and illustrates the vulnerability of the publication process that, in addition to the quality of the science, is influenced by undeclared or unknown personal, financial and social factors.</p><p>In 2004, 10 co-authors of Wakefield issued a personal retraction. The <i>Lancet</i> finally retracted the paper in 2010, 12 years later. In 2009, the Special Master of the US Vaccine Injury Compensation Court had issued a final opinion in the Omnibus Autism Proceedings about the lack of causal relation between MMR vaccine and autism in a class action. One hoped that the MMR-autism scare was finally over. However, as Jonathan Swift put it some time ago: ‘Falsehood flies and the truth comes limping after; so that when men come to be undeceived it is too late: the jest is over and the tale has had its effect’ (Swift, <span>1710</span>).</p><p>MMR uptake has continued to be low in many countries, often lower than the 95% uptake level necessary to consolidate herd immunity. Even small % decrements in MMR uptake are associated with large increases in measles cases and rising financial health costs (Lo &amp; Hotez, <span>2017</span>). Fear of autism remains the first concern in the general public (Novilla et al., <span>2023</span>), and in families within the autism community (Fombonne et al., <span>2020</span>) resulting in MMR immunization delays/refusals in vaccinating younger siblings of children with autism (Zerbo et al., <span>2018</span>). Vaccine refusal has existed since the invention of smallpox vaccine by Jenner in 1796. Dr. Moseley, an anti-vaxxer pioneer, warned of bestiality and human-cow hybrids (vaccine comes from the latin <i>vacca</i> that means cow), persuaded that ‘owing to vaccination the British ladies might wander in the fields to receive the embraces of the bull’. The profile of modern anti-vaxxers, often recruited among celebrities, has evolved and frequently contains features such as distrust of science and of expertise and an inclination for conspiracy theories (in a study of online information on the topic of autism and vaccine, Donald Trump was the name most often appearing on Web pages mentioning vaccine and autism (Arif et al., <span>2018</span>)).</p><p>To describe the persisting reluctance to embrace life-saving, cost-effective, preventive measures (MMR or other vaccines), researchers now use the terminology of ‘vaccine hesitancy’ (VH) that appeared in the early 2010s and is an attenuated form of extreme anti-vaccine positions, equivalent to a broader anti-vax phenotype. WHO recently designated VH as one of the top 10 threats to be urgently combated. Yet, its contours are difficult to discern since it is not a unitary concept, rather it is a convenient descriptive term for a very heterogeneous group of people lying at the extreme of a distribution. Across VH individuals, variable constellations of arguments are found involving anti-vaccines sentiment, suspicion of evidence-based medicine, beliefs in the sanctity of mother nature, lack of science understanding, skepticism about experts, more extreme religious beliefs, distrust in government programs, low sense of social responsibility, a tendency to embrace conspiracy theories, fear of side effects (autism or else) <i>together</i> with actual decision/behaviors resulting in refusal or delay of immunizations. The latter behavioral part (vaccine refusal/delay) is what unifies VH individuals who otherwise became part of the VH subculture through multiple different pathways. Adding to this heterogeneity of motives, the focus of hesitancy varies according to vaccine type (HPV, MMR, and COVID-19), presumed adverse consequence (autism, auto-immune disorders), or vaccine ingredients (aluminum adjuvant, formaldehyde, thimerosal, etc.) (DeStefano, Bodenstab, &amp; Offit, <span>2019</span>). Here, we mostly confine our comments to the presumed MMR-autism links because of their direct relevance to mental health. A recent review of VH questionnaires emphasizes the measurement challenges that VH presents (Oduwole, Pienaar, Mahomed, &amp; Wiysonge, <span>2022</span>), owing to the diversity of reasons and foci among individuals with VH. Perhaps, separating more clearly the outcome of VH from its determinants will assist the optimization of these tools.</p><p>I became concerned that the use by researchers of VH terminology (including by me) has elevated VH to a higher status than it merits. By measuring and studying it, we substantiated it as a separate valid construct, a distinct phenotype of vulnerable subjects, almost amounting to a new personality or anxiety/stress syndrome that deserves special and delicate handling. As a result, health care providers give VH parents extra time and consideration, accept to post-pone an MMR immunization or to deliver its three components in separate shots (a practice known to contribute to incomplete immunizations), or now even train to motivational interviewing techniques to reduce ‘hesitancy.’ When vaccinations mandates have been relaxed (as in California or Texas), the generous distribution of non-medical vaccine exemptions for <i>personal belief</i> to VH parents has created community pockets of unvaccinated children from where outbreaks flared (Bednarczyk, King, Lahijani, &amp; Omer, <span>2019</span>). The direct and indirect costs of VH have been soaring for individuals and society.</p><p>A remarkable asymmetry exists. No equivalent attention is given to increased risks and costs incurred by non-VH parents as a result of VH, not only for their own vaccinated children but for the community where they live. Infants, immuno-suppressed children, children with vaccination failures, and those from underserved social groups with limited access to health care are unnecessarily exposed and at high risk of contracting measles. Yet, the costs attributable to VH are shared in full by those who followed vaccination recommendations. All public health programs have met with resistance by a minority, for example, restricting smoking in public places or wearing safety belts. However, one should note that there is no syndrome of ‘safety-belt ambivalence’ that could prevent the unattached driver to be fined; nor simply invoking the right to personal freedom will result in clemency. Likewise, smokers often have to pay a higher insurance premium. While coercion is not advisable, some form of personal accountability for VH might be considered.</p><p>In the United Kingdom, it took two decades for vaccination rates to recover. During that time there were 12,000 cases of measles, hundreds of hospitalizations, and several preventable deaths. Europe experienced a fourfold increase in measles cases and 35 deaths in 2017 alone. The incidence of measles in the United States was 0.03/100,000 in 2000 and rose to 0.39/100,000 in 2019. The year 2019 was the worst year for measles in the United States since 1992, with 1,282 cases reported, the vast majority among unvaccinated people. The forecasts for 2024 are worse.</p><p>Measles does not cause autism in children. But it kills them.</p>","PeriodicalId":187,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry","volume":"65 11","pages":"1403-1406"},"PeriodicalIF":6.5000,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jcpp.14058","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jcpp.14058","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHIATRY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Measles are back. Larger and more frequent measles outbreaks have been reported in 2024 in the United Kingdom and the United States, which predated the COVID-19 pandemic. Measles deaths worldwide rose to an estimated 136,000 in 2022, mostly children.

Immunizations have been one of the major contributor to the 20th century increased life expectancy alongside better nutrition, hygiene, and lifestyle, way ahead of medical technological prowess that keeps impressing us. Vaccination campaigns and other preventive policies (e.g. car safety belts and anti-tobacco campaigns) have saved and continue to save lives and reduce morbidity (remember tuberculosis or poliomyelitis) much more than da Vinci surgical robots or heart transplants. Vaccines are safe and post-licensure vaccine safety is continuously monitored with different overlapping population-based surveillance systems (Buttery & Clothier, 2022). Vaccines achieved the total eradication of smallpox in the late 1970s. A worldwide campaign to eradicate measles was well under way in the 1990s. Then, a case series published in a prestigious medical journal triggered fears of vaccine-induced autism in the public. Despite rigorous science rapidly dismissing the original claim, the measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccine scare and fear of autism propagated. Years later, it has morphed into what we now refer to as ‘vaccine hesitancy’ (VH), MMR vaccine uptake is still below optimal levels, fueling ongoing measles outbreaks. How did we get there?

The saga started in 1998 with an editorial decision by the Lancet to publish a case series of 12 children suggesting a new syndrome of autism triggered by MMR vaccination had been discovered (Wakefield et al., 1998). The publication of a case series in the Lancet almost seems a contradiction in terms. As researchers and editors, we know how cautious we must remain before drawing causal inferences between two variables. There is an established hierarchy of research designs based on their relative strengths for causality assessment. Experimental designs are at the top (e.g. the RCT), followed by controlled observational (cohort and case–control) studies, and by much weaker ecological studies (relying on confounded correlations between group-level data); at the very bottom, lies the case series which, at most, helps generate hypotheses (especially when the knowledge base is quasi-inexistant) but is never sufficient to test causal ones.

While it is fair to assume that those who reviewed the 1998 manuscript and those who made the decision to publish it were unaware at the time of the fraudulent nature of the data (see: Godlee, Smith, and Marcovitch (2011); Deer 2011a, 2011b and Deer (2020)), multiple red flags were readily noticeable: its first author had spent previous years trying to prove that adult inflammatory bowel disorders were linked to measles vaccine, in studies that were unreplicated and dismissed by academic peers; having 12 children with autism ‘referred’ to an adult gastro-enterological surgeon was suspicious; no co-author had autism expertise; there was no control group of any sort; the MMR-autism connection entirely relied on unverified retrospective parental accounts asserting onset of autism days after MMR immunization in a previously normally developing child; regressive autism was presented as a novel syndrome (some Kanner's cases experienced developmental regression and loss of skills); implausible persistence of the measles virus in gut tissues. These accounts were unreliable and insufficient to speculate about a novel etiology of autism.

Despite numerous failures in the integrity of the peer review and editorial process (Deer, 2020), the predicted high impact of this article influenced the Lancet decision to publish. At JCPP, too, editors consider the potential impact of submitted articles as a criterion favoring acceptance for publication based on estimating the resulting increment in scientific knowledge. Instead, Wakefield et al. (1998)'s impact consisted of sensationalistic journalism, greatness of its author and institutional prestige, with disregard for the predictable negative public health consequences. By 2004–2005, the scientific community had conducted several independent population-based controlled studies that all failed to support a link between MMR vaccine and autism (I summarized them in Plotkin et al., 2020). Anecdotally, in one of these studies, we (with colleagues at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine) completed a large case–control study in England on the putative links between MMR vaccine and autism. It takes years to complete such studies and when in 2003 we submitted our manuscript to the Lancet, the journal (with the same editor) first returned it without review (‘it is no longer an interesting hypothesis’!). Our study had compared 1,294 autism cases to 4,469 matched controls and showed no association. This negative study was significant for public health but it was unworthy of media coverage. It took a persuasive phone call from one of us to challenge the initial rejection, have the manuscript sent out for review and finally published (Smeeth et al., 2004). The contrast between the editorial paths of these two manuscripts is striking and illustrates the vulnerability of the publication process that, in addition to the quality of the science, is influenced by undeclared or unknown personal, financial and social factors.

In 2004, 10 co-authors of Wakefield issued a personal retraction. The Lancet finally retracted the paper in 2010, 12 years later. In 2009, the Special Master of the US Vaccine Injury Compensation Court had issued a final opinion in the Omnibus Autism Proceedings about the lack of causal relation between MMR vaccine and autism in a class action. One hoped that the MMR-autism scare was finally over. However, as Jonathan Swift put it some time ago: ‘Falsehood flies and the truth comes limping after; so that when men come to be undeceived it is too late: the jest is over and the tale has had its effect’ (Swift, 1710).

MMR uptake has continued to be low in many countries, often lower than the 95% uptake level necessary to consolidate herd immunity. Even small % decrements in MMR uptake are associated with large increases in measles cases and rising financial health costs (Lo & Hotez, 2017). Fear of autism remains the first concern in the general public (Novilla et al., 2023), and in families within the autism community (Fombonne et al., 2020) resulting in MMR immunization delays/refusals in vaccinating younger siblings of children with autism (Zerbo et al., 2018). Vaccine refusal has existed since the invention of smallpox vaccine by Jenner in 1796. Dr. Moseley, an anti-vaxxer pioneer, warned of bestiality and human-cow hybrids (vaccine comes from the latin vacca that means cow), persuaded that ‘owing to vaccination the British ladies might wander in the fields to receive the embraces of the bull’. The profile of modern anti-vaxxers, often recruited among celebrities, has evolved and frequently contains features such as distrust of science and of expertise and an inclination for conspiracy theories (in a study of online information on the topic of autism and vaccine, Donald Trump was the name most often appearing on Web pages mentioning vaccine and autism (Arif et al., 2018)).

To describe the persisting reluctance to embrace life-saving, cost-effective, preventive measures (MMR or other vaccines), researchers now use the terminology of ‘vaccine hesitancy’ (VH) that appeared in the early 2010s and is an attenuated form of extreme anti-vaccine positions, equivalent to a broader anti-vax phenotype. WHO recently designated VH as one of the top 10 threats to be urgently combated. Yet, its contours are difficult to discern since it is not a unitary concept, rather it is a convenient descriptive term for a very heterogeneous group of people lying at the extreme of a distribution. Across VH individuals, variable constellations of arguments are found involving anti-vaccines sentiment, suspicion of evidence-based medicine, beliefs in the sanctity of mother nature, lack of science understanding, skepticism about experts, more extreme religious beliefs, distrust in government programs, low sense of social responsibility, a tendency to embrace conspiracy theories, fear of side effects (autism or else) together with actual decision/behaviors resulting in refusal or delay of immunizations. The latter behavioral part (vaccine refusal/delay) is what unifies VH individuals who otherwise became part of the VH subculture through multiple different pathways. Adding to this heterogeneity of motives, the focus of hesitancy varies according to vaccine type (HPV, MMR, and COVID-19), presumed adverse consequence (autism, auto-immune disorders), or vaccine ingredients (aluminum adjuvant, formaldehyde, thimerosal, etc.) (DeStefano, Bodenstab, & Offit, 2019). Here, we mostly confine our comments to the presumed MMR-autism links because of their direct relevance to mental health. A recent review of VH questionnaires emphasizes the measurement challenges that VH presents (Oduwole, Pienaar, Mahomed, & Wiysonge, 2022), owing to the diversity of reasons and foci among individuals with VH. Perhaps, separating more clearly the outcome of VH from its determinants will assist the optimization of these tools.

I became concerned that the use by researchers of VH terminology (including by me) has elevated VH to a higher status than it merits. By measuring and studying it, we substantiated it as a separate valid construct, a distinct phenotype of vulnerable subjects, almost amounting to a new personality or anxiety/stress syndrome that deserves special and delicate handling. As a result, health care providers give VH parents extra time and consideration, accept to post-pone an MMR immunization or to deliver its three components in separate shots (a practice known to contribute to incomplete immunizations), or now even train to motivational interviewing techniques to reduce ‘hesitancy.’ When vaccinations mandates have been relaxed (as in California or Texas), the generous distribution of non-medical vaccine exemptions for personal belief to VH parents has created community pockets of unvaccinated children from where outbreaks flared (Bednarczyk, King, Lahijani, & Omer, 2019). The direct and indirect costs of VH have been soaring for individuals and society.

A remarkable asymmetry exists. No equivalent attention is given to increased risks and costs incurred by non-VH parents as a result of VH, not only for their own vaccinated children but for the community where they live. Infants, immuno-suppressed children, children with vaccination failures, and those from underserved social groups with limited access to health care are unnecessarily exposed and at high risk of contracting measles. Yet, the costs attributable to VH are shared in full by those who followed vaccination recommendations. All public health programs have met with resistance by a minority, for example, restricting smoking in public places or wearing safety belts. However, one should note that there is no syndrome of ‘safety-belt ambivalence’ that could prevent the unattached driver to be fined; nor simply invoking the right to personal freedom will result in clemency. Likewise, smokers often have to pay a higher insurance premium. While coercion is not advisable, some form of personal accountability for VH might be considered.

In the United Kingdom, it took two decades for vaccination rates to recover. During that time there were 12,000 cases of measles, hundreds of hospitalizations, and several preventable deaths. Europe experienced a fourfold increase in measles cases and 35 deaths in 2017 alone. The incidence of measles in the United States was 0.03/100,000 in 2000 and rose to 0.39/100,000 in 2019. The year 2019 was the worst year for measles in the United States since 1992, with 1,282 cases reported, the vast majority among unvaccinated people. The forecasts for 2024 are worse.

Measles does not cause autism in children. But it kills them.

社论:自闭症病例系列、疫苗犹豫不决和麻疹致死
因此,医疗服务提供者会给 VH 家长更多的时间和考虑,同意推迟麻腮风疫苗的免疫接种,或将其三个组成部分分开注射(众所周知,这种做法会导致免疫接种不完全),现在甚至会培训动机访谈技术以减少 "犹豫"。当疫苗接种规定放宽时(如在加利福尼亚州或得克萨斯州),出于个人信仰向 VH 父母慷慨发放非医疗性疫苗豁免,造成了社区内未接种疫苗儿童的小规模爆发(Bednarczyk, King, Lahijani, &amp; Omer, 2019)。对个人和社会而言,VH 的直接和间接成本一直在飙升。非 VH 父母因 VH 而增加的风险和成本没有得到同等的关注,这些风险和成本不仅对他们自己已接种疫苗的孩子,而且对他们生活的社区都造成了影响。婴儿、免疫抑制儿童、疫苗接种失败的儿童,以及那些来自服务不足、医疗条件有限的社会群体的儿童,都不必要地暴露在麻疹疫情中,并面临着感染麻疹的高风险。然而,因 VH 而产生的费用却全部由那些遵循疫苗接种建议的人分担。所有公共卫生计划都会遇到少数人的抵制,例如限制在公共场所吸烟或系安全带。然而,我们应该注意到,"安全带矛盾综合症 "并不能阻止不系安全带的司机被罚款;仅仅援引个人自由权也不会导致宽大处理。同样,吸烟者往往要支付更高的保险费。虽然强迫是不可取的,但可以考虑对 VH 实行某种形式的个人问责制。在此期间,出现了 12,000 例麻疹病例,数百人住院治疗,还有几例可预防的死亡。仅在 2017 年,欧洲的麻疹病例就增加了四倍,死亡人数达到 35 人。2000 年,美国的麻疹发病率为 0.03/100,000,2019 年上升到 0.39/100,000。2019年是美国自1992年以来麻疹最严重的一年,共报告了1282例病例,绝大多数是未接种疫苗的人。预计 2024 年的情况会更糟。麻疹不会导致儿童自闭症。麻疹不会导致儿童自闭症,但会导致他们死亡。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
13.80
自引率
5.30%
发文量
169
审稿时长
1 months
期刊介绍: The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry (JCPP) is a highly regarded international publication that focuses on the fields of child and adolescent psychology and psychiatry. It is recognized for publishing top-tier, clinically relevant research across various disciplines related to these areas. JCPP has a broad global readership and covers a diverse range of topics, including: Epidemiology: Studies on the prevalence and distribution of mental health issues in children and adolescents. Diagnosis: Research on the identification and classification of childhood disorders. Treatments: Psychotherapeutic and psychopharmacological interventions for child and adolescent mental health. Behavior and Cognition: Studies on the behavioral and cognitive aspects of childhood disorders. Neuroscience and Neurobiology: Research on the neural and biological underpinnings of child mental health. Genetics: Genetic factors contributing to the development of childhood disorders. JCPP serves as a platform for integrating empirical research, clinical studies, and high-quality reviews from diverse perspectives, theoretical viewpoints, and disciplines. This interdisciplinary approach is a key feature of the journal, as it fosters a comprehensive understanding of child and adolescent mental health. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry is published 12 times a year and is affiliated with the Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health (ACAMH), which supports the journal's mission to advance knowledge and practice in the field of child and adolescent mental health.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信