Approach versus avoidance and the polarity principle-On an unrecognized ambiguity of the approach/avoidance paradigm.

IF 4.6 Q2 MATERIALS SCIENCE, BIOMATERIALS
Dirk Wentura, Andrea Paulus
{"title":"Approach versus avoidance and the polarity principle-On an unrecognized ambiguity of the approach/avoidance paradigm.","authors":"Dirk Wentura, Andrea Paulus","doi":"10.1037/xhp0001247","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study examined the role of polarity correspondence (Proctor & Cho, 2006) in the approach/avoidance task. It was hypothesized that the typically found approach/avoidance effect could (at least in part) be explained by matching polarities of the stimuli and the response alternatives. To test this hypothesis, polarity of the stimuli was manipulated in three experiments. Experiment 1 showed that two neutral categories elicited an approach/avoidance asymmetry similar to that typically found for positive and negative stimuli when the categorization of stimuli was framed as \"yes (Category A)\" versus \"no (not Category A).\" This pattern is explained by assuming a polarity match between the \"yes\" category and the approach response. Experiment 2 used positive (flowers) versus negative (insects) categories. In a control condition, a typical compatibility effect was found (i.e., positive [negative] items relatively facilitated approach [avoidance]). However, when the task consisted of categorizing insects as the + polarity (\"yes, insect\" vs. \"no, no insect\"), the compatibility effect reversed; it was significantly increased when flowers were the \"yes\" category. In Experiment 3, polarity of positive/negative stimuli (flowers/insects) was manipulated prior to completion of a standard approach/avoidance task with flowers and insects as stimuli. Approximately the same pattern of results (albeit less pronounced) was found as in Experiment 2. The results suggest that results with the approach/avoidance task interpreted in terms of valence or motivational relevance may be (partly) due to polarity differences. This should be taken into account if these effects are routinely interpreted in terms of valence or motivational relevance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":2,"journal":{"name":"ACS Applied Bio Materials","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6000,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"ACS Applied Bio Materials","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0001247","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"MATERIALS SCIENCE, BIOMATERIALS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

The present study examined the role of polarity correspondence (Proctor & Cho, 2006) in the approach/avoidance task. It was hypothesized that the typically found approach/avoidance effect could (at least in part) be explained by matching polarities of the stimuli and the response alternatives. To test this hypothesis, polarity of the stimuli was manipulated in three experiments. Experiment 1 showed that two neutral categories elicited an approach/avoidance asymmetry similar to that typically found for positive and negative stimuli when the categorization of stimuli was framed as "yes (Category A)" versus "no (not Category A)." This pattern is explained by assuming a polarity match between the "yes" category and the approach response. Experiment 2 used positive (flowers) versus negative (insects) categories. In a control condition, a typical compatibility effect was found (i.e., positive [negative] items relatively facilitated approach [avoidance]). However, when the task consisted of categorizing insects as the + polarity ("yes, insect" vs. "no, no insect"), the compatibility effect reversed; it was significantly increased when flowers were the "yes" category. In Experiment 3, polarity of positive/negative stimuli (flowers/insects) was manipulated prior to completion of a standard approach/avoidance task with flowers and insects as stimuli. Approximately the same pattern of results (albeit less pronounced) was found as in Experiment 2. The results suggest that results with the approach/avoidance task interpreted in terms of valence or motivational relevance may be (partly) due to polarity differences. This should be taken into account if these effects are routinely interpreted in terms of valence or motivational relevance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

接近与回避以及极性原则--接近/回避范式中未被认识到的模糊之处。
本研究考察了极性对应(Proctor 和 Cho,2006 年)在接近/回避任务中的作用。研究假设,通常发现的接近/回避效应(至少部分)可以通过刺激物和反应选择的极性匹配来解释。为了验证这一假设,我们在三个实验中操纵了刺激物的极性。实验 1 显示,当刺激的分类框架为 "是(A 类)"与 "否(非 A 类)"时,两个中性类别引起的接近/回避不对称性类似于正反面刺激的典型情况。这种模式可以通过假设 "是 "的类别与接近反应之间的极性匹配来解释。实验 2 采用了正面(花卉)和负面(昆虫)类别。在对照条件下,发现了典型的相容性效应(即正面[负面]项目相对促进接近[回避])。然而,当任务包括将昆虫归为 + 极性("是,昆虫 "与 "否,没有昆虫")时,相容性效应发生了逆转;当花卉归为 "是 "类时,相容性效应显著增加。在实验 3 中,在完成以鲜花和昆虫为刺激物的标准接近/回避任务之前,对正面/负面刺激物(鲜花/昆虫)的极性进行了操作。结果发现与实验 2 大致相同(尽管不那么明显)。这些结果表明,以情感或动机相关性来解释接近/回避任务的结果可能(部分)是由于极性差异造成的。如果这些效应被常规地用情绪或动机相关性来解释,则应考虑到这一点。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
ACS Applied Bio Materials
ACS Applied Bio Materials Chemistry-Chemistry (all)
CiteScore
9.40
自引率
2.10%
发文量
464
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信