A vision for a diverse, inclusive, equitable, and representative developmental science

IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL
Leher Singh
{"title":"A vision for a diverse, inclusive, equitable, and representative developmental science","authors":"Leher Singh","doi":"10.1111/desc.13548","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The fundamental goals of a well-ordered science are to describe, predict, and explain. Developmental scientists seek to apply these basic goals to the study of change over time. Developmental scientists have made momentous strides in understanding sources, mechanisms, and drivers of age-related change. Since its inception, <i>Developmental Science</i> has prioritized cross-cutting research with a focus on developmental mechanisms, contributing to 15 years of exceptionally high-impact and transformative research.</p><p>As our field advances, like many domains of Psychology, developmental science confronts fundamental questions about the generalizability of our empirical record. Our ability to address these critical questions significantly impacts our utility and credibility as a discipline. Essential to a generalizable science of human development is diversification of samples, populations, environments, and epistemologies that define our research endeavors. In recent years, there have been several calls for greater participant diversity given that developmental research has traditionally drawn disproportionately from geographically and socioculturally narrow samples (e.g., Nielsen et al., <span>2017</span>; Singh, Cristia et al., <span>2023</span>). Findings from nonrepresentative slices of the global population have often been advanced as universal even though much of the world is entirely absent from our empirical record (Henrich et al., <span>2010</span>).</p><p>Efforts to diversify samples and to broaden scientific approaches have transformed our knowledge of even the most basic of developmental processes. For example, it was commonly believed that infants developed the capacity for independent locomotion in a fixed developmental sequence, reified in universal guidelines for early motor development published by the World Health Organization. However, these assumptions have been challenged by studying infants from understudied physical and social ecologies. Developmental scientists conducted studies in Tajikistan, where infants have different affordances for independent locomotion than infants in the United States (U.S). Tajik infants are bound supine in a <i>gahvora</i> cradle for significant amounts of time (Karasik et al., <span>2023</span>). Confinement of movement at a stage of active motor exploration is contraindicated by public health advice offered in the US to caregivers (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, <span>2024</span>). Thus, by US cultural norms, Tajik infants might be expected to lag behind U.S. infants. Indeed, Tajik infants, when assessed using U.S. norms, appear to do so. However, unlike U.S. infants, as early as 3 years of age, Tajik infants climb high ladders, sit perched on high ledges, and manipulate sharp tools, demonstrating a different set of motor abilities that do not surface on any U.S. motor assessment tool (Karasik et al., <span>2019</span>). Importantly, long-term motor outcomes do not differ between US and Tajik infants (Karasik et al., <span>2023</span>). This provides an example of how different sociocultural ecologies produce different affordances, which drives developmental adaptation. This example further demonstrates that investigations of varied sociocultural ecologies are critical for theoretical and empirical advancement.</p><p>In this editorial, I examine core questions of diversity, equity and inclusion and fundamentally of representation and scientific generalizability with the goal of deepening our scientific understanding of mechanisms of development. I focus on three critical areas: foregrounding context and culture in development; fostering inclusive scientific participation; and examining the intersection of open science practices and goals for a diverse, inclusive, and equitable science.</p><p>The author declares no conflicts of interest with regard to the funding source for this study.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":"27 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.1000,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13548","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Developmental Science","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/desc.13548","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

The fundamental goals of a well-ordered science are to describe, predict, and explain. Developmental scientists seek to apply these basic goals to the study of change over time. Developmental scientists have made momentous strides in understanding sources, mechanisms, and drivers of age-related change. Since its inception, Developmental Science has prioritized cross-cutting research with a focus on developmental mechanisms, contributing to 15 years of exceptionally high-impact and transformative research.

As our field advances, like many domains of Psychology, developmental science confronts fundamental questions about the generalizability of our empirical record. Our ability to address these critical questions significantly impacts our utility and credibility as a discipline. Essential to a generalizable science of human development is diversification of samples, populations, environments, and epistemologies that define our research endeavors. In recent years, there have been several calls for greater participant diversity given that developmental research has traditionally drawn disproportionately from geographically and socioculturally narrow samples (e.g., Nielsen et al., 2017; Singh, Cristia et al., 2023). Findings from nonrepresentative slices of the global population have often been advanced as universal even though much of the world is entirely absent from our empirical record (Henrich et al., 2010).

Efforts to diversify samples and to broaden scientific approaches have transformed our knowledge of even the most basic of developmental processes. For example, it was commonly believed that infants developed the capacity for independent locomotion in a fixed developmental sequence, reified in universal guidelines for early motor development published by the World Health Organization. However, these assumptions have been challenged by studying infants from understudied physical and social ecologies. Developmental scientists conducted studies in Tajikistan, where infants have different affordances for independent locomotion than infants in the United States (U.S). Tajik infants are bound supine in a gahvora cradle for significant amounts of time (Karasik et al., 2023). Confinement of movement at a stage of active motor exploration is contraindicated by public health advice offered in the US to caregivers (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2024). Thus, by US cultural norms, Tajik infants might be expected to lag behind U.S. infants. Indeed, Tajik infants, when assessed using U.S. norms, appear to do so. However, unlike U.S. infants, as early as 3 years of age, Tajik infants climb high ladders, sit perched on high ledges, and manipulate sharp tools, demonstrating a different set of motor abilities that do not surface on any U.S. motor assessment tool (Karasik et al., 2019). Importantly, long-term motor outcomes do not differ between US and Tajik infants (Karasik et al., 2023). This provides an example of how different sociocultural ecologies produce different affordances, which drives developmental adaptation. This example further demonstrates that investigations of varied sociocultural ecologies are critical for theoretical and empirical advancement.

In this editorial, I examine core questions of diversity, equity and inclusion and fundamentally of representation and scientific generalizability with the goal of deepening our scientific understanding of mechanisms of development. I focus on three critical areas: foregrounding context and culture in development; fostering inclusive scientific participation; and examining the intersection of open science practices and goals for a diverse, inclusive, and equitable science.

The author declares no conflicts of interest with regard to the funding source for this study.

Abstract Image

发展科学多元化、包容性、公平性和代表性的愿景。
有序科学的基本目标是描述、预测和解释。发育科学家试图将这些基本目标应用于对随时间变化的研究。发育科学家在了解与年龄有关的变化的来源、机制和驱动因素方面取得了长足的进步。自成立以来,发展科学一直以发展机制为重点,将横向研究作为优先事项,在 15 年的时间里开展了极具影响力和变革性的研究。随着我们这一领域的发展,与心理学的许多领域一样,发展科学也面临着关于我们的经验记录是否具有普遍性的基本问题。我们解决这些关键问题的能力极大地影响了我们作为一门学科的实用性和可信度。对于人类发展科学的普适性而言,至关重要的是样本、人群、环境和认识论的多样化,这也是我们研究工作的基础。近年来,鉴于发展研究历来不成比例地从地域和社会文化上狭窄的样本中汲取营养,人们多次呼吁提高参与者的多样性(例如,Nielsen 等人,2017 年;Singh, Cristia 等人,2023 年)。尽管我们的经验记录中完全没有世界上大部分地区的数据,但来自非代表性的全球人口切片的研究结果却常常被认为具有普遍性(Henrich 等人,2010 年)。例如,人们普遍认为婴儿是按照固定的发育顺序发展独立运动能力的,世界卫生组织发布的早期运动发育通用指南也重申了这一点。然而,通过研究未被充分研究的自然和社会生态环境中的婴儿,这些假设受到了挑战。发育科学家在塔吉克斯坦进行了研究,那里的婴儿与美国的婴儿有不同的独立运动能力。塔吉克斯坦的婴儿在相当长的时间里都是仰卧在 gahvora 摇篮里的(Karasik 等人,2023 年)。美国的公共卫生建议禁止在婴儿积极探索运动的阶段限制其运动(美国疾病控制和预防中心,2024 年)。因此,按照美国的文化标准,塔吉克斯坦婴儿可能会落后于美国婴儿。事实上,如果按照美国的标准进行评估,塔吉克斯坦婴儿似乎确实落后于美国婴儿。然而,与美国婴儿不同的是,早在 3 岁时,塔吉克斯坦婴儿就会爬上高梯子、坐在高高的窗台上、摆弄锋利的工具,表现出一系列不同的运动能力,而这些能力在任何美国运动评估工具中都没有体现出来(Karasik et al.)重要的是,美国和塔吉克斯坦婴儿的长期运动结果并无不同(Karasik 等人,2023 年)。这提供了一个例子,说明不同的社会文化生态如何产生不同的承受能力,从而推动发展适应。在这篇社论中,我从根本上探讨了多样性、公平性和包容性以及代表性和科学普适性等核心问题,目的是加深我们对发展机制的科学理解。我重点关注三个关键领域:强调发展的背景和文化;促进包容性的科学参与;研究开放科学实践与多样化、包容性和公平科学目标之间的交叉。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
8.10
自引率
8.10%
发文量
132
期刊介绍: Developmental Science publishes cutting-edge theory and up-to-the-minute research on scientific developmental psychology from leading thinkers in the field. It is currently the only journal that specifically focuses on human developmental cognitive neuroscience. Coverage includes: - Clinical, computational and comparative approaches to development - Key advances in cognitive and social development - Developmental cognitive neuroscience - Functional neuroimaging of the developing brain
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信