Dutch speakers take referent predictability into account, irrespective of addressee presence

J. Vogels
{"title":"Dutch speakers take referent predictability into account, irrespective of addressee presence","authors":"J. Vogels","doi":"10.5070/g6011197","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Language comprehension involves continuously making predictions about what will be mentioned next. If speakers take these predictions into account, one would expect that they try to be extra clear (e.g., by saying “the girl with the big earrings”) when they are going to say something less predictable. Conversely, speakers do not need to be as clear when the listener already expects the thing that they are about to mention, and can therefore suffice with a pronoun such as she. Previous research testing this hypothesis has found mixed results, with some studies finding that the referent’s predictability in discourse affects pronoun use and others finding that it does not. One explanation might be that speakers are more likely to take predictability into account when there is a co-present addressee who is predicting the next referent. To test this possibility, I conducted a language production experiment in which participants produced spoken continuations of narrative fragments. The fragments were accompanied by pictures that made clear how the story continued. Half of the participants performed the task without anyone else being present, while the other half told the stories to another person, who had to pick out the correct picture. Referent predictability was varied by manipulating the coherence relation in the narrative context. In addition, I calculated a surprisal score for each character in each narrative, as a more direct measure of its predictability. The results showed that with higher predictability, speakers were indeed more likely to use a pronoun than a definite NP to refer to the target character in their continuations. However, it did not matter whether the speaker was telling the stories to a co-present addressee or not. The results are discussed in light of accounts that distinguish between taking the perspective of a specific and that of a hypothetical listener.","PeriodicalId":491939,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"63 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2024-04-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","FirstCategoryId":"0","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g6011197","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Language comprehension involves continuously making predictions about what will be mentioned next. If speakers take these predictions into account, one would expect that they try to be extra clear (e.g., by saying “the girl with the big earrings”) when they are going to say something less predictable. Conversely, speakers do not need to be as clear when the listener already expects the thing that they are about to mention, and can therefore suffice with a pronoun such as she. Previous research testing this hypothesis has found mixed results, with some studies finding that the referent’s predictability in discourse affects pronoun use and others finding that it does not. One explanation might be that speakers are more likely to take predictability into account when there is a co-present addressee who is predicting the next referent. To test this possibility, I conducted a language production experiment in which participants produced spoken continuations of narrative fragments. The fragments were accompanied by pictures that made clear how the story continued. Half of the participants performed the task without anyone else being present, while the other half told the stories to another person, who had to pick out the correct picture. Referent predictability was varied by manipulating the coherence relation in the narrative context. In addition, I calculated a surprisal score for each character in each narrative, as a more direct measure of its predictability. The results showed that with higher predictability, speakers were indeed more likely to use a pronoun than a definite NP to refer to the target character in their continuations. However, it did not matter whether the speaker was telling the stories to a co-present addressee or not. The results are discussed in light of accounts that distinguish between taking the perspective of a specific and that of a hypothetical listener.
荷兰语使用者会考虑指代的可预测性,而不论受话人是否在场
语言理解包括不断预测接下来会提到什么。如果说话者考虑到了这些预测,那么当他们要说的东西不太容易预测时,就会尽量说得更清楚(例如,说 "戴大耳环的女孩")。相反,当听者已经预料到他们将要提到的事情时,说话者就不需要说得那么清楚,因此用她这样的代词就足够了。以前对这一假设进行测试的研究结果不一,有些研究发现,话语中所指代词的可预测性会影响代词的使用,而有些研究则发现不会。一种解释可能是,当有一个共同在场的对象在预测下一个指称时,说话人更有可能将可预测性考虑在内。为了验证这种可能性,我做了一个语言生产实验,让参与者用口语续写叙述片段。这些片段配有图片,清楚地说明了故事是如何继续的。一半参与者在没有其他人在场的情况下完成任务,另一半参与者则把故事讲给另一个人听,后者必须选出正确的图片。通过操纵叙事语境中的连贯关系来改变参照物的可预测性。此外,我还为每个故事中的每个人物计算了意外得分,以此更直接地衡量其可预测性。结果表明,如果可预测性越高,说话者确实越倾向于在续篇中使用代词而不是定语从句来指称目标人物。但是,这与说话者是否在向共同在场的对象讲述故事无关。我们将根据区分特定听者视角和假设听者视角的观点来讨论这些结果。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信