Are rules meant to be broken? When and why consistent rule-following undermines versus enhances trust

IF 3.2 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL
Michael W. White , Emma E. Levine , Alexander C. Kristal
{"title":"Are rules meant to be broken? When and why consistent rule-following undermines versus enhances trust","authors":"Michael W. White ,&nbsp;Emma E. Levine ,&nbsp;Alexander C. Kristal","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2023.104552","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Although consistency has long been positioned as a cornerstone of trust, the present paper examines when and why consistent rule-following undermines versus enhances trust. Across six preregistered experiments (total </span><em>N</em> = 2649), we study trust in decision-makers (e.g., police officers, managers) who either consistently punish offenders according to codified rules (e.g., laws, policies) or who exercise discretion by occasionally deviating from rules. We find that people are more likely to trust decision-makers that exercise discretion rather than consistently follow rules, to the extent that discretion signals benevolence. The degree to which discretion is perceived as benevolent, and therefore trustworthy, depends on what type of discretion is exercised, how the decision is reached, to whom discretion is applied, and the nature of the transgressions being punished. Specifically, people reward decision-makers who use discretion leniently (rather than punitively) and apply it thoughtfully (rather than arbitrarily). Moreover, only certain cases of punishment are deemed appropriate for discretion. When discretion is perceived to be motivated by favoritism because it is applied to close others, or when the basis for discretion is unclear because there is little variance in cases of the crime being punished, discretion fails to signal benevolence and elicit trust. This research has important implications for understanding trust, discretion, and the reputational consequences of punishment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"111 ","pages":"Article 104552"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2000,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103123001099","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Although consistency has long been positioned as a cornerstone of trust, the present paper examines when and why consistent rule-following undermines versus enhances trust. Across six preregistered experiments (total N = 2649), we study trust in decision-makers (e.g., police officers, managers) who either consistently punish offenders according to codified rules (e.g., laws, policies) or who exercise discretion by occasionally deviating from rules. We find that people are more likely to trust decision-makers that exercise discretion rather than consistently follow rules, to the extent that discretion signals benevolence. The degree to which discretion is perceived as benevolent, and therefore trustworthy, depends on what type of discretion is exercised, how the decision is reached, to whom discretion is applied, and the nature of the transgressions being punished. Specifically, people reward decision-makers who use discretion leniently (rather than punitively) and apply it thoughtfully (rather than arbitrarily). Moreover, only certain cases of punishment are deemed appropriate for discretion. When discretion is perceived to be motivated by favoritism because it is applied to close others, or when the basis for discretion is unclear because there is little variance in cases of the crime being punished, discretion fails to signal benevolence and elicit trust. This research has important implications for understanding trust, discretion, and the reputational consequences of punishment.

规则是用来打破的吗?何时以及为什么一贯遵守规则会破坏信任,而不是增强信任
尽管一致性长期以来一直被定位为信任的基石,但本文探讨了一致性规则遵循何时以及为什么会破坏信任而不是增强信任。在6个预先登记的实验(总N = 2649)中,我们研究了对决策者(例如,警察,管理人员)的信任,这些决策者要么始终根据成文规则(例如,法律,政策)惩罚违法者,要么偶尔偏离规则行使自由裁量权。我们发现,人们更有可能信任行使自由裁量权的决策者,而不是始终如一地遵守规则,在某种程度上,自由裁量权标志着仁慈。自由裁量权在多大程度上被认为是仁慈的,因此是值得信赖的,取决于行使何种类型的自由裁量权,如何做出决定,对谁实行自由裁量权,以及被惩罚的违法行为的性质。具体来说,人们会奖励那些宽松地(而不是惩罚性地)使用自由裁量权并深思熟虑地(而不是武断地)运用它的决策者。此外,只有某些惩罚案件才被认为是适当的裁量权。当自由裁量权被认为是出于偏袒的动机,因为它适用于关系密切的人,或者当自由裁量权的基础不明确,因为在惩罚犯罪的情况下几乎没有差异,自由裁量权就不能发出仁慈的信号,也不能引起信任。本研究对理解惩罚的信任、自由裁量权和声誉后果具有重要意义。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
6.30
自引率
2.90%
发文量
134
期刊介绍: The Journal of Experimental Social Psychology publishes original research and theory on human social behavior and related phenomena. The journal emphasizes empirical, conceptually based research that advances an understanding of important social psychological processes. The journal also publishes literature reviews, theoretical analyses, and methodological comments.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信