The Missing Mind: Contrasting Civilization with Non-civilization Development and Functioning

D. Narvaez, Mary S. Tarsha
{"title":"The Missing Mind: Contrasting Civilization with Non-civilization Development and Functioning","authors":"D. Narvaez, Mary S. Tarsha","doi":"10.4324/9781003056140-5","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"ing. Differentiation makes distinctions, defining each thing as one thing only. Univocity relies on dualistic, dichotomous logic (something is or isn’t), emphasizing causes and effects which rely on linear thinking. The sense of the present is minimal as people are caught in trying to predict the future based on what was noticed from the past. Obsession with order, precision and prediction becomes normalized—all left hemisphere concerns (McGilchrist, 2009). These skills are valuable and necessary for both an individual and community to solve problems but according to McGilchrist and others (e.g., Tweedy, 2021) become distorted without the help of the right hemisphere’s global integration. As a result of obsessive differentiation (sorting and naming separate objects), civilization creates a singular world with a hierarchical shape, a pyramid of order based on linguistic structure (subject-predicate-object) that becomes the model for logic (universal statement →particular statement→conclusion) and is transformed into social law. For Bram, hypotactic (hierarchically-arranged) societies are made up of bits, separate entities, whether people, offices, assembly lines, or power structures. Persistent differentiation, encouraged by (noun-based Indo-European) language and law, leads to a hierarchical society that uses contests to determine who reaches the top of the pyramid. Contest winners take the top position—or top abstraction in fields of study like science, or in realms of life like religion or schooling. Univocity plays a significant role in keeping the domination hierarchy in place to benefit those with more privilege. As ecological feminists have documented about Western domination culture (e.g., Warren, 2000), people get categorized, abstracted and coerced into their place. Punishments become part of life and orderliness becomes paramount. Life becomes about staying in your place and carrying your load. The clash of these vastly different consciousnesses was apparent when Europeans explorers, settlers and anthropologists encountered societies existing primarily in polysemy, but also in paradox – that is, a combination of diffuse or peripheral awareness in combination with focused attention or mental alertness (Berman, 2000). The Europeans were confused by community members’ lack of precise definitions, shifting stories and social configurations, and their lack of leaders. They were not “logical” in the linear, univocal sense. At the same time, First Nation peoples remarked on the soullessness of European invaders, their inflexibility and lack of openness and awareness of a sentient Earth (Narvaez, 2019). Showing similarities to Bram’s analysis, anthropologist E. Richard Sorenson (1998) noted a “preconquest consciousness” (versus postconquest consciousness in westernized nations) among the different Indigenous Peoples with whom he lived around the world over decades. 1 Among those with preconquest consciousness, Sorenson documented the shifting descriptions of self, others and places, based on context. There were no fixed identities. So, for example, a person could have multiple names that came or went, whose use shifted with the desire of mates or context. With the lack of fixed or rigid identities, the overall sense of the world was not static but dynamic, with actions changing and dependent on foregrounding, activity, season and mood, rather than on fixed cognitive thought. The difference between univocity and polysemic consciousnesses appears to align with left hemisphere-dominated and integrated modes of being, respectively. Also similar to Bram’s analysis, McGilchrist (2009) reviewed empirical research on left and right brain functioning and then concluded that a lack of hemispheric integration (minimal right hemisphere involvement) and left-brain dominance has characterized most of Western civilization. Left-brain dominated thought is governed by the explicit and the disembodied, oriented to static, categorizable things, interested only in what is preconceived or has a defined purpose. It focuses on detail, mechanistic processes, perfectionistic thinking and is largely more comfortable with impersonal interactions. The left brain contains more myelination within itself, indicating its function to refer to information that is known (self-referring), unlike the right brain whose expanded myelination and connectivity to the whole brain allows for perception and intake of new information outside itself, playing an integrative role in broad, generalized thinking (Tucker, Roth & Blair, 1986). The left-hemisphere’s skills are not in and of themselves deleterious. Rather, they are enhanced and balanced only when in dialogue with the right hemisphere (Tweedy, 2021). The right hemisphere is able to ground and make sense of the detailed, perfectionistic and impersonal insights provided by the left. When a bi-hemisphere collaboration occurs, one maintains the ‘big picture,’ responds to an evolving, dynamic, interconnected, world and is comfortable with obscurity (vagueness or mystery) and the personal, maintaining connections with the natural world and other systems (McGilchrist, 2009). 1 Sorenson used film to discover what he experienced rather than his notes from contemporaneous experience because the latter were biased by his postconquest consciousness. He is credited with starting anthropological filmography and the Human Studies Film Archives at the Smithsonian. According to McGilchrist, capacities for the diffuse, wide-open attention especially declined with the ascent of the Enlightenment and the Western. The lack of hemispheric integration coupled with lopsided hemisphere leadership (left over right), led to an emphasis on materialism at the expense of social cohesion and respect for individuality—the unique nature of each person was replaced with categorical descriptions, such as race or socioeconomic status. McGilchrist points out that the common modern condition—feeling fragmented, devitalized, depersonalized, depressed, or dissociated, along with lost emotional depth and empathy— correspond to an overbearing left hemisphere and underactive right hemisphere—what psychotherapy seeks to rebalance (Tweedy, 2021). Berman (2000) suggests that a large part of western civilization reflects disconnection from nature and from flexible coordination with others, including the other than human. In our view, the lack of connection to others, including to the natural world, generates further disconnections within the individual as well as the individual’s understanding of present and future life. According to Bram (2018), persistent differentiation leads to deep anxiety and death terror as the sense of the present is thin and emptied out of creative polysemy and the focus is on the future which will bring about certain death. In contrast, integration of hemispheric functioning, the right-brain leading, results in thinking that is open and receptive, attuned to the energies of the moment, dominated by a feeling of relation and multiperspectivalism, interested in penetrating mysteries and that which is beyond apparent perception or comprehension; such integration is apparent in noncivilized communities the world over (Berman, 2000; Descola, 2013; Wolff, 2000). This type of hemisphere integration is able to drink deeply of the realities of the present moment, participatory consciousness, as well as conceive, creatively and realistically, the future. Moreover, brains that are integrated across hemispheres are associated with compassionate and kind action (Tweedy, 2021). Like Sorenson, McGilchrist, Berman and Bram, physicist David Bohm (1994) also described two types of awareness. The first, insight-intelligence, the source of creativity, is open to the flow of shared being with others outside the self. It taps into transrational reality where participation is entwined with observation, instilling a sense of awe. For westernized minds, it appears in eureka moments. Among First Nation societies, it is routinely experienced in dreams, visions and intuitions (Wolff, 2001). For others, in participatory ceremonies, rituals and dances, sometimes with mind-altering practices. Bohm’s second form of awareness is thought-in-themind, a dualistic, subject-object worldview familiar to westerners. It consists of static habits of mind, a fossilized consciousness, such as beliefs and other self-referential or cultural loops. It is the self-looping of the left hemisphere. Cognitively speaking, we can see a lining up of polysemy with insight-intelligence and the right hemisphere, and a lining up of univocity with left hemisphere self-referential thought. Taken together, we suggest a correspondence between the socio-cultural transformation and the biopsychosocial. That is, diminishing polysemy with the rise of patriarchy and the devaluing of women and natural cycles may be caused by the devaluation and degradation of nurturing child formation. We propose that a significant causal factor in bringing about the emphasis on univocity is the “misraising of the species’ brain,” that is, the lack of EDN provision, leading to an underdevelopment of coordinated connection to others and righthemispheric functions generally. Specifically, it is our hypothesis that civilizations’ concern for controlling nature and non-elites led to a diminishment of EDN provision which became culturally sanctioned, leading to an enhancement of left-hemisphere functions at the expense of right hemisphere functions. The shift away from the EDN is particularly hard on males. Lack of EDN provision impairs healthy development of all people but critically impacts developing males because neurobiologically and psychologically they develop more slowly, have less built-in resilience and need more nurturing than girls (Schore, 2017). Without appropriate nurturing boys more easily develop dysregulation in multiple ways, including an easy downshifting to primitive survival systems","PeriodicalId":389401,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Cognitive Archaeology","volume":"33 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2021-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"1","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Psychology and Cognitive Archaeology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003056140-5","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

Abstract

ing. Differentiation makes distinctions, defining each thing as one thing only. Univocity relies on dualistic, dichotomous logic (something is or isn’t), emphasizing causes and effects which rely on linear thinking. The sense of the present is minimal as people are caught in trying to predict the future based on what was noticed from the past. Obsession with order, precision and prediction becomes normalized—all left hemisphere concerns (McGilchrist, 2009). These skills are valuable and necessary for both an individual and community to solve problems but according to McGilchrist and others (e.g., Tweedy, 2021) become distorted without the help of the right hemisphere’s global integration. As a result of obsessive differentiation (sorting and naming separate objects), civilization creates a singular world with a hierarchical shape, a pyramid of order based on linguistic structure (subject-predicate-object) that becomes the model for logic (universal statement →particular statement→conclusion) and is transformed into social law. For Bram, hypotactic (hierarchically-arranged) societies are made up of bits, separate entities, whether people, offices, assembly lines, or power structures. Persistent differentiation, encouraged by (noun-based Indo-European) language and law, leads to a hierarchical society that uses contests to determine who reaches the top of the pyramid. Contest winners take the top position—or top abstraction in fields of study like science, or in realms of life like religion or schooling. Univocity plays a significant role in keeping the domination hierarchy in place to benefit those with more privilege. As ecological feminists have documented about Western domination culture (e.g., Warren, 2000), people get categorized, abstracted and coerced into their place. Punishments become part of life and orderliness becomes paramount. Life becomes about staying in your place and carrying your load. The clash of these vastly different consciousnesses was apparent when Europeans explorers, settlers and anthropologists encountered societies existing primarily in polysemy, but also in paradox – that is, a combination of diffuse or peripheral awareness in combination with focused attention or mental alertness (Berman, 2000). The Europeans were confused by community members’ lack of precise definitions, shifting stories and social configurations, and their lack of leaders. They were not “logical” in the linear, univocal sense. At the same time, First Nation peoples remarked on the soullessness of European invaders, their inflexibility and lack of openness and awareness of a sentient Earth (Narvaez, 2019). Showing similarities to Bram’s analysis, anthropologist E. Richard Sorenson (1998) noted a “preconquest consciousness” (versus postconquest consciousness in westernized nations) among the different Indigenous Peoples with whom he lived around the world over decades. 1 Among those with preconquest consciousness, Sorenson documented the shifting descriptions of self, others and places, based on context. There were no fixed identities. So, for example, a person could have multiple names that came or went, whose use shifted with the desire of mates or context. With the lack of fixed or rigid identities, the overall sense of the world was not static but dynamic, with actions changing and dependent on foregrounding, activity, season and mood, rather than on fixed cognitive thought. The difference between univocity and polysemic consciousnesses appears to align with left hemisphere-dominated and integrated modes of being, respectively. Also similar to Bram’s analysis, McGilchrist (2009) reviewed empirical research on left and right brain functioning and then concluded that a lack of hemispheric integration (minimal right hemisphere involvement) and left-brain dominance has characterized most of Western civilization. Left-brain dominated thought is governed by the explicit and the disembodied, oriented to static, categorizable things, interested only in what is preconceived or has a defined purpose. It focuses on detail, mechanistic processes, perfectionistic thinking and is largely more comfortable with impersonal interactions. The left brain contains more myelination within itself, indicating its function to refer to information that is known (self-referring), unlike the right brain whose expanded myelination and connectivity to the whole brain allows for perception and intake of new information outside itself, playing an integrative role in broad, generalized thinking (Tucker, Roth & Blair, 1986). The left-hemisphere’s skills are not in and of themselves deleterious. Rather, they are enhanced and balanced only when in dialogue with the right hemisphere (Tweedy, 2021). The right hemisphere is able to ground and make sense of the detailed, perfectionistic and impersonal insights provided by the left. When a bi-hemisphere collaboration occurs, one maintains the ‘big picture,’ responds to an evolving, dynamic, interconnected, world and is comfortable with obscurity (vagueness or mystery) and the personal, maintaining connections with the natural world and other systems (McGilchrist, 2009). 1 Sorenson used film to discover what he experienced rather than his notes from contemporaneous experience because the latter were biased by his postconquest consciousness. He is credited with starting anthropological filmography and the Human Studies Film Archives at the Smithsonian. According to McGilchrist, capacities for the diffuse, wide-open attention especially declined with the ascent of the Enlightenment and the Western. The lack of hemispheric integration coupled with lopsided hemisphere leadership (left over right), led to an emphasis on materialism at the expense of social cohesion and respect for individuality—the unique nature of each person was replaced with categorical descriptions, such as race or socioeconomic status. McGilchrist points out that the common modern condition—feeling fragmented, devitalized, depersonalized, depressed, or dissociated, along with lost emotional depth and empathy— correspond to an overbearing left hemisphere and underactive right hemisphere—what psychotherapy seeks to rebalance (Tweedy, 2021). Berman (2000) suggests that a large part of western civilization reflects disconnection from nature and from flexible coordination with others, including the other than human. In our view, the lack of connection to others, including to the natural world, generates further disconnections within the individual as well as the individual’s understanding of present and future life. According to Bram (2018), persistent differentiation leads to deep anxiety and death terror as the sense of the present is thin and emptied out of creative polysemy and the focus is on the future which will bring about certain death. In contrast, integration of hemispheric functioning, the right-brain leading, results in thinking that is open and receptive, attuned to the energies of the moment, dominated by a feeling of relation and multiperspectivalism, interested in penetrating mysteries and that which is beyond apparent perception or comprehension; such integration is apparent in noncivilized communities the world over (Berman, 2000; Descola, 2013; Wolff, 2000). This type of hemisphere integration is able to drink deeply of the realities of the present moment, participatory consciousness, as well as conceive, creatively and realistically, the future. Moreover, brains that are integrated across hemispheres are associated with compassionate and kind action (Tweedy, 2021). Like Sorenson, McGilchrist, Berman and Bram, physicist David Bohm (1994) also described two types of awareness. The first, insight-intelligence, the source of creativity, is open to the flow of shared being with others outside the self. It taps into transrational reality where participation is entwined with observation, instilling a sense of awe. For westernized minds, it appears in eureka moments. Among First Nation societies, it is routinely experienced in dreams, visions and intuitions (Wolff, 2001). For others, in participatory ceremonies, rituals and dances, sometimes with mind-altering practices. Bohm’s second form of awareness is thought-in-themind, a dualistic, subject-object worldview familiar to westerners. It consists of static habits of mind, a fossilized consciousness, such as beliefs and other self-referential or cultural loops. It is the self-looping of the left hemisphere. Cognitively speaking, we can see a lining up of polysemy with insight-intelligence and the right hemisphere, and a lining up of univocity with left hemisphere self-referential thought. Taken together, we suggest a correspondence between the socio-cultural transformation and the biopsychosocial. That is, diminishing polysemy with the rise of patriarchy and the devaluing of women and natural cycles may be caused by the devaluation and degradation of nurturing child formation. We propose that a significant causal factor in bringing about the emphasis on univocity is the “misraising of the species’ brain,” that is, the lack of EDN provision, leading to an underdevelopment of coordinated connection to others and righthemispheric functions generally. Specifically, it is our hypothesis that civilizations’ concern for controlling nature and non-elites led to a diminishment of EDN provision which became culturally sanctioned, leading to an enhancement of left-hemisphere functions at the expense of right hemisphere functions. The shift away from the EDN is particularly hard on males. Lack of EDN provision impairs healthy development of all people but critically impacts developing males because neurobiologically and psychologically they develop more slowly, have less built-in resilience and need more nurturing than girls (Schore, 2017). Without appropriate nurturing boys more easily develop dysregulation in multiple ways, including an easy downshifting to primitive survival systems
缺失的心灵:对比文明与非文明的发展和功能
当双半球合作发生时,一个人保持“大局”,对一个不断发展的、动态的、相互联系的世界做出反应,并对模糊(模糊或神秘)和个人感到舒适,保持与自然世界和其他系统的联系(McGilchrist, 2009)。1 .索伦森用电影来发现他所经历的,而不是从同时代的经验中记录下来,因为后者被他的后征服意识所偏见。他被认为是人类学电影编目和史密森学会人类研究电影档案馆的奠基人。麦吉尔克里斯特认为,随着启蒙运动和西方文化的兴起,分散的、开放的注意力的能力尤其下降。由于大脑半球缺乏整合,再加上大脑半球的领导不平衡(左半球比右半球),导致了以牺牲社会凝聚力和对个性的尊重为代价来强调物质主义——每个人的独特性被种族或社会经济地位等分类描述所取代。麦克吉尔克里斯特指出,现代社会常见的状况——感觉支离破碎、失去活力、失去个性、抑郁或分离,以及失去情感深度和同理心——与霸道的左半球和不活跃的右半球相对应,而这正是心理治疗试图重新平衡的(Tweedy, 2021)。Berman(2000)认为,西方文明的很大一部分反映了与自然的脱节,以及与他人(包括人类以外的人)的灵活协调。在我们看来,缺乏与他人的联系,包括与自然世界的联系,会在个人内部产生进一步的脱节,以及个人对现在和未来生活的理解。Bram(2018)认为,持续的分化会导致深深的焦虑和死亡恐惧,因为当下的感觉很薄,缺乏创造性的多义性,关注的是未来,这将带来一定的死亡。相比之下,大脑半球功能的整合,即右脑的主导,导致思维的开放和接受性,与当下的能量相协调,被一种关系和多视角的感觉所主导,对穿透奥秘和超越表面感知或理解的事物感兴趣;这种融合在世界各地的非文明社区中很明显(Berman, 2000;Descola, 2013;沃尔夫,2000)。这种类型的半球整合能够深刻地汲取当下的现实,参与意识,以及创造性地和现实地构想未来。此外,跨半球整合的大脑与富有同情心和善良的行为有关(Tweedy, 2021)。与Sorenson、McGilchrist、Berman和Bram一样,物理学家David Bohm(1994)也描述了两种类型的意识。第一种,洞察力-智力,创造力的源泉,是开放的,与自我之外的其他人共享。它利用了跨理性的现实,参与与观察交织在一起,灌输了一种敬畏感。对于西化的人来说,它出现在灵光一现的时刻。在第一民族社会中,它经常出现在梦境、幻象和直觉中(Wolff, 2001)。对另一些人来说,在参与性的仪式、仪式和舞蹈中,有时会有改变思维的练习。波姆的第二种意识形式是“心中思考”,这是一种西方人所熟悉的主客体二元世界观。它包括静态的思维习惯,僵化的意识,如信仰和其他自我参照或文化循环。它是左半球的自我循环。从认知上讲,我们可以看到多义性与洞察力和右半球排列在一起,单一性与左半球自我参照思维排列在一起。综上所述,我们认为社会文化转型与生物心理社会转型之间存在对应关系。也就是说,随着父权制的兴起和对妇女和自然周期的贬低,多义性的减少可能是由于对养育儿童形成的贬低和退化造成的。我们提出,强调单一性的一个重要原因是“物种大脑的错误养育”,即缺乏EDN供应,导致与他人的协调连接和右半球功能普遍发育不足。具体来说,我们的假设是,文明对控制自然和非精英的关注导致了EDN供应的减少,这成为文化上的认可,导致左半球功能的增强以右半球功能为代价。从EDN的转变对男性来说尤其艰难。缺乏EDN会损害所有人的健康发展,但会严重影响发育中的男性,因为在神经生物学和心理上,他们比女孩发育更慢,缺乏内在的弹性,需要更多的培育(Schore, 2017)。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信