It's not just what we don't know: The mapping problem in the acquisition of negation

IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY
Victor Gomes , Rebecca Doherty , Daniel Smits , Susan Goldin-Meadow , John C. Trueswell , Roman Feiman
{"title":"It's not just what we don't know: The mapping problem in the acquisition of negation","authors":"Victor Gomes ,&nbsp;Rebecca Doherty ,&nbsp;Daniel Smits ,&nbsp;Susan Goldin-Meadow ,&nbsp;John C. Trueswell ,&nbsp;Roman Feiman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101592","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How do learners learn what <em>no</em> and <em>not</em> mean when they are only presented with what is? Given its complexity, abstractness, and roles in logic, truth-functional negation might be a conceptual accomplishment. As a result, young children’s gradual acquisition of negation words might be due to their undergoing a gradual conceptual change that is necessary to represent those words’ logical meaning. However, it’s also possible that linguistic expressions of negation take time to learn because of children’s gradually increasing grasp of their language. To understand what <em>no</em> and <em>not</em> mean, children might first need to understand the rest of the sentences in which those words are used. We provide experimental evidence that conceptually equipped learners (adults) face the same acquisition challenges that children do when their access to linguistic information is restricted, which simulates how much language children understand at different points in acquisition. When watching a silenced video of naturalistic uses of negators by parents speaking to their children, adults could tell when the parent was prohibiting the child and struggled with inferring that negators were used to express logical negation. However, when provided with additional information about what else the parent said, guessing that the parent had expressed logical negation became easy for adults. Though our findings do not rule out that young learners also undergo conceptual change, they show that increasing understanding of language alone, with no accompanying conceptual change, can account for the gradual acquisition of negation words.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101592"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0000,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Cognitive Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010028523000506","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

How do learners learn what no and not mean when they are only presented with what is? Given its complexity, abstractness, and roles in logic, truth-functional negation might be a conceptual accomplishment. As a result, young children’s gradual acquisition of negation words might be due to their undergoing a gradual conceptual change that is necessary to represent those words’ logical meaning. However, it’s also possible that linguistic expressions of negation take time to learn because of children’s gradually increasing grasp of their language. To understand what no and not mean, children might first need to understand the rest of the sentences in which those words are used. We provide experimental evidence that conceptually equipped learners (adults) face the same acquisition challenges that children do when their access to linguistic information is restricted, which simulates how much language children understand at different points in acquisition. When watching a silenced video of naturalistic uses of negators by parents speaking to their children, adults could tell when the parent was prohibiting the child and struggled with inferring that negators were used to express logical negation. However, when provided with additional information about what else the parent said, guessing that the parent had expressed logical negation became easy for adults. Though our findings do not rule out that young learners also undergo conceptual change, they show that increasing understanding of language alone, with no accompanying conceptual change, can account for the gradual acquisition of negation words.

这不仅仅是我们不知道的:否定习得中的映射问题。
当学习者只被告知“是”时,他们如何学习“否”和“否”的含义?考虑到其复杂性、抽象性和在逻辑中的作用,真值功能否定可能是一种概念上的成就。因此,幼儿对否定词的逐渐习得可能是由于他们经历了一个渐进的概念变化,这是表达这些词的逻辑意义所必需的。然而,由于孩子们对语言的掌握程度逐渐提高,否定的语言表达也可能需要时间来学习。为了理解“不”和“不”的意思,孩子们可能首先需要理解这些单词所用的其余句子。我们提供的实验证据表明,当儿童获得语言信息受到限制时,具备概念的学习者(成年人)面临着与儿童相同的习得挑战,这模拟了儿童在不同的习得点上理解了多少语言。当观看父母对孩子说话时自然使用否定词的无声视频时,成年人可以判断出父母何时禁止孩子,并努力推断否定词是用来表达逻辑否定的。然而,当被提供关于父母还说了什么的额外信息时,成年人很容易猜测父母表达了合乎逻辑的否定。尽管我们的研究结果并不排除年轻学习者也会经历概念变化,但它们表明,只有对语言的理解不断增加,而没有伴随的概念变化,才能解释否定词的逐渐习得。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Cognitive Psychology
Cognitive Psychology 医学-心理学
CiteScore
5.40
自引率
3.80%
发文量
29
审稿时长
50 days
期刊介绍: Cognitive Psychology is concerned with advances in the study of attention, memory, language processing, perception, problem solving, and thinking. Cognitive Psychology specializes in extensive articles that have a major impact on cognitive theory and provide new theoretical advances. Research Areas include: • Artificial intelligence • Developmental psychology • Linguistics • Neurophysiology • Social psychology.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信