Ryan A. Millager , Mary S. Dietrich , Robin M. Jones
{"title":"Behavioral and cognitive-affective features of stuttering in preschool-age children: Regression and exploratory cluster analyses","authors":"Ryan A. Millager , Mary S. Dietrich , Robin M. Jones","doi":"10.1016/j.jfludis.2023.105972","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>The purpose of this study was to investigate associations among behavioral and cognitive-affective features of stuttering in preschool-age children who stutter, and the extent to which participants may or may not cluster together based on multiple indices of stuttering.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Participants were 296 preschool-age children who stutter (mean age 47.9 months). Correlation and regression analyses, as well as k-means cluster analyses were conducted between and among several indices of stuttering: frequency of stuttering- and non-stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs and NSLDs), ratios of repetitions and prolongations/blocks out of total number of SLDs, associated nonspeech behaviors, duration of stuttering events, KiddyCAT scores (Vanryckeghem & Brutten, 2007), and a TOCS parent-rated scale (Gillam et al., 2009).</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>For preschool-age children who stutter, most indices of overt stuttering behaviors were intercorrelated (e.g., more SLDs were associated with higher ratio of repetitions). Self-reported KiddyCAT scores (Vanryckeghem & Brutten, 2007) were largely not significantly associated with stuttering. Cluster analyses yielded two participant groupings: a larger group with less prominent stuttering features and a smaller group with more prominent features.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>This study contributes to an increasingly comprehensive and nuanced understanding of the heterogeneous features of stuttering and their development in preschool-age children. Findings show strong intercorrelations between measures of stuttering behaviors, but more tenuous relationships between behaviors and cognitive-affective reactions to stuttering. Exploration of clusters of characteristics within this population revealed potential opportunities for future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49166,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Fluency Disorders","volume":"76 ","pages":"Article 105972"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1000,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10330204/pdf/","citationCount":"1","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Fluency Disorders","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0094730X23000153","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Abstract
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to investigate associations among behavioral and cognitive-affective features of stuttering in preschool-age children who stutter, and the extent to which participants may or may not cluster together based on multiple indices of stuttering.
Methods
Participants were 296 preschool-age children who stutter (mean age 47.9 months). Correlation and regression analyses, as well as k-means cluster analyses were conducted between and among several indices of stuttering: frequency of stuttering- and non-stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs and NSLDs), ratios of repetitions and prolongations/blocks out of total number of SLDs, associated nonspeech behaviors, duration of stuttering events, KiddyCAT scores (Vanryckeghem & Brutten, 2007), and a TOCS parent-rated scale (Gillam et al., 2009).
Results
For preschool-age children who stutter, most indices of overt stuttering behaviors were intercorrelated (e.g., more SLDs were associated with higher ratio of repetitions). Self-reported KiddyCAT scores (Vanryckeghem & Brutten, 2007) were largely not significantly associated with stuttering. Cluster analyses yielded two participant groupings: a larger group with less prominent stuttering features and a smaller group with more prominent features.
Conclusions
This study contributes to an increasingly comprehensive and nuanced understanding of the heterogeneous features of stuttering and their development in preschool-age children. Findings show strong intercorrelations between measures of stuttering behaviors, but more tenuous relationships between behaviors and cognitive-affective reactions to stuttering. Exploration of clusters of characteristics within this population revealed potential opportunities for future research.
目的:本研究的目的是调查学龄前儿童口吃的行为和认知情感特征之间的关系,以及参与者根据多种口吃指数可能聚集在一起或不聚集在一起的程度。方法:参与者为296名学龄前口吃儿童(平均年龄47.9个月)。在口吃的几个指标之间进行了相关和回归分析,以及k均值聚类分析:口吃和非口吃样障碍(SLD和NSLD)的频率、重复和延长/阻塞占SLD总数的比率、相关的非口吃行为、口吃事件的持续时间,KiddyCAT评分(Vanryckeghem&Brutten,2007)和TOCS父母评分量表(Gillam et al.,2009)。自行报告的KiddyCAT评分(Vanryckeghem&Brutten,2007)在很大程度上与口吃无关。聚类分析产生了两个参与者分组:一个较大的口吃特征不太突出的组和一个较小的口吃特征更突出的组。结论:这项研究有助于更全面、更细致地了解学龄前儿童口吃的异质性特征及其发展。研究结果表明,口吃行为的测量之间有很强的相互关联,但行为和对口吃的认知情感反应之间的关系更为脆弱。对这一群体特征集群的探索揭示了未来研究的潜在机会。
期刊介绍:
Journal of Fluency Disorders provides comprehensive coverage of clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects of stuttering, including the latest remediation techniques. As the official journal of the International Fluency Association, the journal features full-length research and clinical reports; methodological, theoretical and philosophical articles; reviews; short communications and much more – all readily accessible and tailored to the needs of the professional.