{"title":"加深对社会联系和社会互动动态的理解","authors":"Andrey E. Ryabinin","doi":"10.1111/gbb.12804","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current volume continues the series of Genes, Brain and Behavior special issues. In the first special issue, we focused on autism and neurodevelopmental disorders.<span><sup>1</sup></span> We concluded the issue with the idea that detailed understanding of these disorders is impossible without profound appreciation of mechanisms underlying natural species-specific social behaviors. Therefore, the current special issue directs readers' attention to fundamentals of social bonding, interactions, group/colony formation and even potential biology of human sociocultural experiences.</p><p>We start with social bonding, or social attachment. This topic has been not only an extensive subject of poetry and fictional literature, but has also been examined across multiple scientific disciplines, including psychology, anthropology, ethology, animal behavior, and cognitive neuroscience. Each of the disciplines provided explanations of this phenomenon in their own terms. A synthesis of these terms would be a substantial intellectual achievement. This need for synthesis is the cornerstone of the review by Gustison and Phelps,<span><sup>2</sup></span> the first article in this issue. The authors explore views on social attachment across multiple disciplines and then focus on pair-bond formation in prairie voles as a case study. The prairie vole (<i>Microtus ochrogaster)</i> is a socially monogamous rodent species. Studies of these species provided much insight into mechanisms of social bonding.<span><sup>3, 4</sup></span> The authors describe the intricate molecular and circuitry mechanisms involved in individual differences in social bonding in prairie voles and provide an example of integration of this knowledge into multi-disciplinary language.</p><p>The next article, a review by Prior et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> provides another type of integration of our understanding of mechanisms involved in social bonding. Specifically, the authors examine how much the social aspects of bond formation are dependent on the involved sensory and perceptual systems. They use the prairie vole and the zebra finch as examples of species, in which neurocircuitry of either chemical or vocal communication, and of social bonding, have been examined. The authors discuss the inter-dependence and bidirectional influences between specific sensory processes and social bonding. This exploration is followed by a framework for future studies in this field.</p><p>The reviews mentioned above are followed by two companion research papers, which challenge the simplified views on processes involved in pair bonding of prairie voles. They provide critical evidence against the idea of potential monomorphism in pair-bonding behaviors in these socially monogamous species. The article by Brusman et al.<span><sup>6</sup></span> compares indices of pair-bonding in the standard “choice” partner preference test<span><sup>7, 8</sup></span> and a novel modified non-choice test across two time points between male and female prairie voles. Using the non-choice test allows authors to document that the increased interaction with the partner versus a novel (“stranger”) vole is driven by female animals. A subsequent social operant version of the procedure expands on this observation by showing that motivation to access a partner is significantly stronger in females than in males.</p><p>In the companion research paper by Vahaba et al.,<span><sup>9</sup></span> authors use their own version of operant social choice task to provide further insight into sex differences in pair bonding in prairie voles. They demonstrate that female voles are willing to work hardest to gain access to their mate versus an unfamiliar male. In contrast, males on average lever-press equally for either their partner or “stranger” female vole. Importantly, this average lack of preference for their mate is not due to potential lack of preference across all males, but is driven by characteristic partner-directed, stranger-directed or indifferent behavior of individual male voles. This observation calls for genetic analysis of differences driving bonding-associated behaviors, which the authors initiate by genotyping the oxytocin receptor gene. Thus, the research papers for Brusman et al. and Vahaba et al. provide striking examples of how a presumably sexually monomorphic social bonding behavior in prairie voles is driven by dimorphic motivational aspects of this behavior.</p><p>Our next article departs from analysis of social bonding in mammals and birds to examine mechanisms of plasticity involved in formation of groups of individuals in a completely different taxon. Namely, Manfredini et al.<span><sup>10</sup></span> examine molecular signatures of plasticity associated with differences in social environment in the fire ants (<i>Solenopsis invicta</i>). Queens of this species can found colonies in either solitary or group conditions.<span><sup>11</sup></span> The researchers examine global co-expression networks in the brain of these queens at two stages of colony founding when they are placed in either solitary or in group conditions. Group conditions of two sizes are examined. Increasing the group sizes results in larger changes in gene expression. The findings indicate that important plasticity changes are associated not only with simple solitary versus groups housing differences but can be also quite striking when associated with apparent subtle differences in population size.</p><p>It is well appreciated that social relationships and interactions are not uniform across individuals within one species. Differences in these interactions can be reflected in social hierarchies. There has been notable interest in examining neural mechanisms contributing to development of dominant versus submissive social status of individuals within species.<span><sup>12</sup></span> However, methods for examining this status vary and can produce varied results across different species and sexes. In their review, Fulenwider et al. examine different methodologies for evaluating social rank across rodent species.<span><sup>13</sup></span> The methodologies are either based on the extent of agonistic behaviors that individual animals exhibit or experience, or on the differences in access to valuable resources by these individuals. Species and sex differences, as well as pitfalls and understudied research areas, are discussed.</p><p>The volume concludes with an article highlighting relationships between molecular mechanisms regulating social behaviors and sociocultural experiences in humans. Lee et al. present a gene-culture coevolution case study of associations between polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor gene and cultural tightness and socio-ecological threats across human populations.<span><sup>14</sup></span> The authors find that cultural tightness and socio-ecological threats are positively correlated with polymorphisms regulating expression of this receptor in the anterior cingulate cortex. These findings provocatively suggest that polymorphisms driving enhanced oxytocin receptor expression underwent positive selection in human populations with tight cultural norms.</p><p>Taken the discussed articles together, as expected, social interactions are governed by exhilaratingly complex multilevel mechanisms. Species differences, sex differences, and individual differences contribute to this complexity. These differences need to be appreciated when examining mechanisms of mental disorders. In the next special issue in this series of Genes, Brain and Behavior, we will continue to present articles on natural social behaviors and transition into models of mental disorders associated with changes in social behaviors.</p>","PeriodicalId":50426,"journal":{"name":"Genes Brain and Behavior","volume":"21 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4000,"publicationDate":"2022-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9744472/pdf/","citationCount":"1","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Deepening the understanding of social bonding and dynamics of social interactions\",\"authors\":\"Andrey E. Ryabinin\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/gbb.12804\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>The current volume continues the series of Genes, Brain and Behavior special issues. In the first special issue, we focused on autism and neurodevelopmental disorders.<span><sup>1</sup></span> We concluded the issue with the idea that detailed understanding of these disorders is impossible without profound appreciation of mechanisms underlying natural species-specific social behaviors. Therefore, the current special issue directs readers' attention to fundamentals of social bonding, interactions, group/colony formation and even potential biology of human sociocultural experiences.</p><p>We start with social bonding, or social attachment. This topic has been not only an extensive subject of poetry and fictional literature, but has also been examined across multiple scientific disciplines, including psychology, anthropology, ethology, animal behavior, and cognitive neuroscience. Each of the disciplines provided explanations of this phenomenon in their own terms. A synthesis of these terms would be a substantial intellectual achievement. This need for synthesis is the cornerstone of the review by Gustison and Phelps,<span><sup>2</sup></span> the first article in this issue. The authors explore views on social attachment across multiple disciplines and then focus on pair-bond formation in prairie voles as a case study. The prairie vole (<i>Microtus ochrogaster)</i> is a socially monogamous rodent species. Studies of these species provided much insight into mechanisms of social bonding.<span><sup>3, 4</sup></span> The authors describe the intricate molecular and circuitry mechanisms involved in individual differences in social bonding in prairie voles and provide an example of integration of this knowledge into multi-disciplinary language.</p><p>The next article, a review by Prior et al.<span><sup>5</sup></span> provides another type of integration of our understanding of mechanisms involved in social bonding. Specifically, the authors examine how much the social aspects of bond formation are dependent on the involved sensory and perceptual systems. They use the prairie vole and the zebra finch as examples of species, in which neurocircuitry of either chemical or vocal communication, and of social bonding, have been examined. The authors discuss the inter-dependence and bidirectional influences between specific sensory processes and social bonding. This exploration is followed by a framework for future studies in this field.</p><p>The reviews mentioned above are followed by two companion research papers, which challenge the simplified views on processes involved in pair bonding of prairie voles. They provide critical evidence against the idea of potential monomorphism in pair-bonding behaviors in these socially monogamous species. The article by Brusman et al.<span><sup>6</sup></span> compares indices of pair-bonding in the standard “choice” partner preference test<span><sup>7, 8</sup></span> and a novel modified non-choice test across two time points between male and female prairie voles. Using the non-choice test allows authors to document that the increased interaction with the partner versus a novel (“stranger”) vole is driven by female animals. A subsequent social operant version of the procedure expands on this observation by showing that motivation to access a partner is significantly stronger in females than in males.</p><p>In the companion research paper by Vahaba et al.,<span><sup>9</sup></span> authors use their own version of operant social choice task to provide further insight into sex differences in pair bonding in prairie voles. They demonstrate that female voles are willing to work hardest to gain access to their mate versus an unfamiliar male. In contrast, males on average lever-press equally for either their partner or “stranger” female vole. Importantly, this average lack of preference for their mate is not due to potential lack of preference across all males, but is driven by characteristic partner-directed, stranger-directed or indifferent behavior of individual male voles. This observation calls for genetic analysis of differences driving bonding-associated behaviors, which the authors initiate by genotyping the oxytocin receptor gene. Thus, the research papers for Brusman et al. and Vahaba et al. provide striking examples of how a presumably sexually monomorphic social bonding behavior in prairie voles is driven by dimorphic motivational aspects of this behavior.</p><p>Our next article departs from analysis of social bonding in mammals and birds to examine mechanisms of plasticity involved in formation of groups of individuals in a completely different taxon. Namely, Manfredini et al.<span><sup>10</sup></span> examine molecular signatures of plasticity associated with differences in social environment in the fire ants (<i>Solenopsis invicta</i>). Queens of this species can found colonies in either solitary or group conditions.<span><sup>11</sup></span> The researchers examine global co-expression networks in the brain of these queens at two stages of colony founding when they are placed in either solitary or in group conditions. Group conditions of two sizes are examined. Increasing the group sizes results in larger changes in gene expression. The findings indicate that important plasticity changes are associated not only with simple solitary versus groups housing differences but can be also quite striking when associated with apparent subtle differences in population size.</p><p>It is well appreciated that social relationships and interactions are not uniform across individuals within one species. Differences in these interactions can be reflected in social hierarchies. There has been notable interest in examining neural mechanisms contributing to development of dominant versus submissive social status of individuals within species.<span><sup>12</sup></span> However, methods for examining this status vary and can produce varied results across different species and sexes. In their review, Fulenwider et al. examine different methodologies for evaluating social rank across rodent species.<span><sup>13</sup></span> The methodologies are either based on the extent of agonistic behaviors that individual animals exhibit or experience, or on the differences in access to valuable resources by these individuals. Species and sex differences, as well as pitfalls and understudied research areas, are discussed.</p><p>The volume concludes with an article highlighting relationships between molecular mechanisms regulating social behaviors and sociocultural experiences in humans. Lee et al. present a gene-culture coevolution case study of associations between polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor gene and cultural tightness and socio-ecological threats across human populations.<span><sup>14</sup></span> The authors find that cultural tightness and socio-ecological threats are positively correlated with polymorphisms regulating expression of this receptor in the anterior cingulate cortex. These findings provocatively suggest that polymorphisms driving enhanced oxytocin receptor expression underwent positive selection in human populations with tight cultural norms.</p><p>Taken the discussed articles together, as expected, social interactions are governed by exhilaratingly complex multilevel mechanisms. Species differences, sex differences, and individual differences contribute to this complexity. These differences need to be appreciated when examining mechanisms of mental disorders. In the next special issue in this series of Genes, Brain and Behavior, we will continue to present articles on natural social behaviors and transition into models of mental disorders associated with changes in social behaviors.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":50426,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Genes Brain and Behavior\",\"volume\":\"21 3\",\"pages\":\"\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.4000,\"publicationDate\":\"2022-03-09\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9744472/pdf/\",\"citationCount\":\"1\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Genes Brain and Behavior\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/gbb.12804\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Genes Brain and Behavior","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/gbb.12804","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
摘要
当前的卷继续一系列的基因,大脑和行为特刊。在第一期特刊中,我们集中讨论了自闭症和神经发育障碍我们的结论是,如果不深入了解自然物种特定社会行为背后的机制,就不可能详细了解这些疾病。因此,本期特刊将引导读者关注社会联系、互动、群体/群体形成甚至人类社会文化经验的潜在生物学基础。我们从社会联系或社会依恋开始。这个话题不仅是诗歌和虚构文学的广泛主题,而且还被多个科学学科所研究,包括心理学、人类学、动物行为学、动物行为学和认知神经科学。每个学科都用自己的术语解释了这一现象。综合这些术语将是一项重大的智力成就。这种综合的需要是Gustison和Phelps的综述的基础,2是本期的第一篇文章。作者探讨了跨多个学科对社会依恋的看法,然后将重点放在草原田鼠的配对纽带形成上作为一个案例研究。草原田鼠(Microtus ochrogaster)是一种社会一夫一妻制的啮齿动物。对这些物种的研究提供了许多关于社会联系机制的见解。3,4作者描述了草原田鼠社会关系中个体差异的复杂分子和电路机制,并提供了将这些知识整合到多学科语言中的一个例子。下一篇文章,由Prior等人撰写的综述提供了另一种类型的整合我们对社会联系机制的理解。具体来说,作者研究了纽带形成的社会方面在多大程度上取决于所涉及的感觉和知觉系统。他们以草原田鼠和斑胸草雀为例,研究了它们的化学或声音交流以及社会联系的神经回路。作者讨论了特定感觉过程与社会联系之间的相互依赖和双向影响。这一探索之后是该领域未来研究的框架。在上述综述之后,有两篇研究论文对草原田鼠配对过程的简化观点提出了挑战。他们提供了关键的证据,反对在这些社会一夫一妻制物种的配对行为中潜在的单态性。Brusman等人的文章(6)比较了标准的“选择”伴侣偏好测试(7,8)和一种新的改进的非选择测试(在两个时间点上)在雄性和雌性草原田鼠之间的配对关系指数。使用非选择测试可以让作者证明,与新(“陌生人”)田鼠相比,与伴侣的互动增加是由雌性动物驱动的。随后的社会操作版本的程序扩展了这一观察结果,表明女性接触伴侣的动机明显强于男性。在Vahaba等人的配套研究论文中,9位作者使用他们自己版本的操作性社会选择任务,进一步深入了解草原田鼠配对关系中的性别差异。它们表明,雌性田鼠愿意付出最大的努力来接近自己的伴侣,而不是陌生的雄性田鼠。相比之下,雄性田鼠对其伴侣或“陌生”雌性田鼠的平均杠杆按压是一样的。重要的是,这种对配偶的平均缺乏偏好并不是由于所有雄性田鼠都潜在地缺乏偏好,而是由个体雄性田鼠的伴侣导向、陌生人导向或冷漠行为所驱动的。这一观察结果要求对驱动结合相关行为的差异进行遗传分析,这是作者通过对催产素受体基因进行分型而开始的。因此,Brusman et al.和Vahaba et al.的研究论文提供了惊人的例子,说明草原田鼠的性单态社会关系行为是如何由这种行为的二态动机方面驱动的。我们的下一篇文章将从对哺乳动物和鸟类社会关系的分析出发,探讨在一个完全不同的分类单元中,个体群体形成的可塑性机制。也就是说,Manfredini等人研究了火蚁(Solenopsis invicta)中与社会环境差异相关的可塑性分子特征。这个物种的蚁后可以在单独或群体的条件下找到殖民地研究人员在蜂群建立的两个阶段检查了这些蜂王大脑中的全球共表达网络,这些蜂王被放置在单独或群体条件下。考察了两种规模的群条件。增加群体大小导致基因表达的更大变化。
Deepening the understanding of social bonding and dynamics of social interactions
The current volume continues the series of Genes, Brain and Behavior special issues. In the first special issue, we focused on autism and neurodevelopmental disorders.1 We concluded the issue with the idea that detailed understanding of these disorders is impossible without profound appreciation of mechanisms underlying natural species-specific social behaviors. Therefore, the current special issue directs readers' attention to fundamentals of social bonding, interactions, group/colony formation and even potential biology of human sociocultural experiences.
We start with social bonding, or social attachment. This topic has been not only an extensive subject of poetry and fictional literature, but has also been examined across multiple scientific disciplines, including psychology, anthropology, ethology, animal behavior, and cognitive neuroscience. Each of the disciplines provided explanations of this phenomenon in their own terms. A synthesis of these terms would be a substantial intellectual achievement. This need for synthesis is the cornerstone of the review by Gustison and Phelps,2 the first article in this issue. The authors explore views on social attachment across multiple disciplines and then focus on pair-bond formation in prairie voles as a case study. The prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster) is a socially monogamous rodent species. Studies of these species provided much insight into mechanisms of social bonding.3, 4 The authors describe the intricate molecular and circuitry mechanisms involved in individual differences in social bonding in prairie voles and provide an example of integration of this knowledge into multi-disciplinary language.
The next article, a review by Prior et al.5 provides another type of integration of our understanding of mechanisms involved in social bonding. Specifically, the authors examine how much the social aspects of bond formation are dependent on the involved sensory and perceptual systems. They use the prairie vole and the zebra finch as examples of species, in which neurocircuitry of either chemical or vocal communication, and of social bonding, have been examined. The authors discuss the inter-dependence and bidirectional influences between specific sensory processes and social bonding. This exploration is followed by a framework for future studies in this field.
The reviews mentioned above are followed by two companion research papers, which challenge the simplified views on processes involved in pair bonding of prairie voles. They provide critical evidence against the idea of potential monomorphism in pair-bonding behaviors in these socially monogamous species. The article by Brusman et al.6 compares indices of pair-bonding in the standard “choice” partner preference test7, 8 and a novel modified non-choice test across two time points between male and female prairie voles. Using the non-choice test allows authors to document that the increased interaction with the partner versus a novel (“stranger”) vole is driven by female animals. A subsequent social operant version of the procedure expands on this observation by showing that motivation to access a partner is significantly stronger in females than in males.
In the companion research paper by Vahaba et al.,9 authors use their own version of operant social choice task to provide further insight into sex differences in pair bonding in prairie voles. They demonstrate that female voles are willing to work hardest to gain access to their mate versus an unfamiliar male. In contrast, males on average lever-press equally for either their partner or “stranger” female vole. Importantly, this average lack of preference for their mate is not due to potential lack of preference across all males, but is driven by characteristic partner-directed, stranger-directed or indifferent behavior of individual male voles. This observation calls for genetic analysis of differences driving bonding-associated behaviors, which the authors initiate by genotyping the oxytocin receptor gene. Thus, the research papers for Brusman et al. and Vahaba et al. provide striking examples of how a presumably sexually monomorphic social bonding behavior in prairie voles is driven by dimorphic motivational aspects of this behavior.
Our next article departs from analysis of social bonding in mammals and birds to examine mechanisms of plasticity involved in formation of groups of individuals in a completely different taxon. Namely, Manfredini et al.10 examine molecular signatures of plasticity associated with differences in social environment in the fire ants (Solenopsis invicta). Queens of this species can found colonies in either solitary or group conditions.11 The researchers examine global co-expression networks in the brain of these queens at two stages of colony founding when they are placed in either solitary or in group conditions. Group conditions of two sizes are examined. Increasing the group sizes results in larger changes in gene expression. The findings indicate that important plasticity changes are associated not only with simple solitary versus groups housing differences but can be also quite striking when associated with apparent subtle differences in population size.
It is well appreciated that social relationships and interactions are not uniform across individuals within one species. Differences in these interactions can be reflected in social hierarchies. There has been notable interest in examining neural mechanisms contributing to development of dominant versus submissive social status of individuals within species.12 However, methods for examining this status vary and can produce varied results across different species and sexes. In their review, Fulenwider et al. examine different methodologies for evaluating social rank across rodent species.13 The methodologies are either based on the extent of agonistic behaviors that individual animals exhibit or experience, or on the differences in access to valuable resources by these individuals. Species and sex differences, as well as pitfalls and understudied research areas, are discussed.
The volume concludes with an article highlighting relationships between molecular mechanisms regulating social behaviors and sociocultural experiences in humans. Lee et al. present a gene-culture coevolution case study of associations between polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor gene and cultural tightness and socio-ecological threats across human populations.14 The authors find that cultural tightness and socio-ecological threats are positively correlated with polymorphisms regulating expression of this receptor in the anterior cingulate cortex. These findings provocatively suggest that polymorphisms driving enhanced oxytocin receptor expression underwent positive selection in human populations with tight cultural norms.
Taken the discussed articles together, as expected, social interactions are governed by exhilaratingly complex multilevel mechanisms. Species differences, sex differences, and individual differences contribute to this complexity. These differences need to be appreciated when examining mechanisms of mental disorders. In the next special issue in this series of Genes, Brain and Behavior, we will continue to present articles on natural social behaviors and transition into models of mental disorders associated with changes in social behaviors.
期刊介绍:
Genes, Brain and Behavior was launched in 2002 with the aim of publishing top quality research in behavioral and neural genetics in their broadest sense. The emphasis is on the analysis of the behavioral and neural phenotypes under consideration, the unifying theme being the genetic approach as a tool to increase our understanding of these phenotypes.
Genes Brain and Behavior is pleased to offer the following features:
8 issues per year
online submissions with first editorial decisions within 3-4 weeks and fast publication at Wiley-Blackwells
High visibility through its coverage by PubMed/Medline, Current Contents and other major abstracting and indexing services
Inclusion in the Wiley-Blackwell consortial license, extending readership to thousands of international libraries and institutions
A large and varied editorial board comprising of international specialists.