记忆与创伤后应激障碍

IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY
H. Flor, M. Wessa
{"title":"记忆与创伤后应激障碍","authors":"H. Flor, M. Wessa","doi":"10.1027/0044-3409/A000012","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"In this issue of the Zeitschrift fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology we have assembled a number of articles and comments that deal with the relationship of trauma and memory, with an emphasis on the role of memory mechanisms in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). There is uniformity in the assumption that memory is severely altered by traumatic events, but memory deficits may also contribute to the development of stress-related disorders. Although there is agreement that in PTSD memory mechanisms are at the core of the disorder, there is disagreement on the nature of the pathology and to what extent additional factors such as appraisal of the deficit are of importance. As Ehlers (2010) notes, there is clear evidence of a preponderance of emotional memories that are often sensory, fast, tied to very aversive moments of the traumatic experience, experienced as immediate rather than past, and related to non-declarative learning processes such as Pavlovian conditioning and priming. Whereas these memories seem to predominate, generalize (see Lissek & Grillon, 2010), and fail to extinguish (Wessa & Flor, 2007), episodic memories of the trauma and life in general seem to be affected, be it by fragmentation (Brewin, 2007), lack of connectedness (Michael, Ehlers, Halligan, & Clark, 2005), or overgeneralization (Schonfeld, Ehlers, Bollinghaus, & Rief, 2007). Early theories about memory mechanisms in PTSD suggested that the documented reduction of hippocampal volume in PTSD might lead to a dissociation of declarative and non-declarative memories (Elzinga & Bremner, 2002). However, these alterations are not as straightforward as it might seem since it could be shown that recall of trauma-related materials is similar in traumatized persons with and without PTSD (Wessa, Jatzko, & Flor, 2006). Retrieval-induced forgetting is also not significantly different in PTSD versus non-PTSD subjects as shown by Koessler and colleagues (2010). Diener, Flor, and Wessa (2010) demonstrate a specific deficit in encoding but not retrieval of episodic memory that is associated with hyperarousal symptoms. This suggests that a very thorough analysis of memory mechanisms is needed and that categories such as declarative/non-declarative or episodic versus semantic need to be further differentiated and specific aspects of these memory processes – such as elaboration, connectedness, or trauma-relevance – need to be considered. For example, the reduced hippocampal volumes could also lead to reduced context conditioning, which is also a form of emotional associative learning, whereas cue conditioning, which is more amygdala-dependent, seems to be enhanced. We (Lang et al., 2009) and others (Alvarez, Biggs, Chen, Pine, & Grillon, 2008; Marschner, Kalisch, Vervliet, Vansteenwegen, & Buchel, 2008) have shown that the hippocampus is involved in context conditioning in human beings and we have preliminary data that indicate impaired context conditioning in persons with small hippocampal volumes (Pohlack, Liebscher, Ridder, Lipinski, & Flor, 2009). A detailed examination of contextual memory in PTSD seems therefore warranted. Context is also important for extinction learning where the organism has to learn to associate the conditioned stimuli with a certain context, no longer with a danger. Bouton and colleagues (2006) and others have emphasized the important role of context for extinction. Disordered context conditioning might therefore not only contribute to the feeling of ‘‘nowness’’ that characterizes the intrusions of PTSD patients and to the inability to feel safe (since safe and dangerous contexts cannot be properly differentiated) but may also impair the extinction of the cue-conditioned fear response since the extinction context has not been associated to the cues. This reasoning is in line with the proposal by Schauer and Elbert (2010), who argue that it is important to consider the peritraumatic defense processes that are activated and that subgroups of patients can be identified that are at different positions in the defense cascade. They specifically emphasize that memory processes are substantially different if persons are putting on a peritraumatic sympathetic activation versus those who went down the entire defense cascade where a parasympathetic dominance and dissociation prevail. This depends on the type of trauma and possibilities of the organism for optimal response. Extinction learning is especially hampered by dissociative","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"217 1","pages":"61-63"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0000,"publicationDate":"2010-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"13","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Memory and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder\",\"authors\":\"H. Flor, M. Wessa\",\"doi\":\"10.1027/0044-3409/A000012\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"In this issue of the Zeitschrift fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology we have assembled a number of articles and comments that deal with the relationship of trauma and memory, with an emphasis on the role of memory mechanisms in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). There is uniformity in the assumption that memory is severely altered by traumatic events, but memory deficits may also contribute to the development of stress-related disorders. Although there is agreement that in PTSD memory mechanisms are at the core of the disorder, there is disagreement on the nature of the pathology and to what extent additional factors such as appraisal of the deficit are of importance. As Ehlers (2010) notes, there is clear evidence of a preponderance of emotional memories that are often sensory, fast, tied to very aversive moments of the traumatic experience, experienced as immediate rather than past, and related to non-declarative learning processes such as Pavlovian conditioning and priming. Whereas these memories seem to predominate, generalize (see Lissek & Grillon, 2010), and fail to extinguish (Wessa & Flor, 2007), episodic memories of the trauma and life in general seem to be affected, be it by fragmentation (Brewin, 2007), lack of connectedness (Michael, Ehlers, Halligan, & Clark, 2005), or overgeneralization (Schonfeld, Ehlers, Bollinghaus, & Rief, 2007). Early theories about memory mechanisms in PTSD suggested that the documented reduction of hippocampal volume in PTSD might lead to a dissociation of declarative and non-declarative memories (Elzinga & Bremner, 2002). However, these alterations are not as straightforward as it might seem since it could be shown that recall of trauma-related materials is similar in traumatized persons with and without PTSD (Wessa, Jatzko, & Flor, 2006). Retrieval-induced forgetting is also not significantly different in PTSD versus non-PTSD subjects as shown by Koessler and colleagues (2010). Diener, Flor, and Wessa (2010) demonstrate a specific deficit in encoding but not retrieval of episodic memory that is associated with hyperarousal symptoms. This suggests that a very thorough analysis of memory mechanisms is needed and that categories such as declarative/non-declarative or episodic versus semantic need to be further differentiated and specific aspects of these memory processes – such as elaboration, connectedness, or trauma-relevance – need to be considered. For example, the reduced hippocampal volumes could also lead to reduced context conditioning, which is also a form of emotional associative learning, whereas cue conditioning, which is more amygdala-dependent, seems to be enhanced. We (Lang et al., 2009) and others (Alvarez, Biggs, Chen, Pine, & Grillon, 2008; Marschner, Kalisch, Vervliet, Vansteenwegen, & Buchel, 2008) have shown that the hippocampus is involved in context conditioning in human beings and we have preliminary data that indicate impaired context conditioning in persons with small hippocampal volumes (Pohlack, Liebscher, Ridder, Lipinski, & Flor, 2009). A detailed examination of contextual memory in PTSD seems therefore warranted. Context is also important for extinction learning where the organism has to learn to associate the conditioned stimuli with a certain context, no longer with a danger. Bouton and colleagues (2006) and others have emphasized the important role of context for extinction. Disordered context conditioning might therefore not only contribute to the feeling of ‘‘nowness’’ that characterizes the intrusions of PTSD patients and to the inability to feel safe (since safe and dangerous contexts cannot be properly differentiated) but may also impair the extinction of the cue-conditioned fear response since the extinction context has not been associated to the cues. This reasoning is in line with the proposal by Schauer and Elbert (2010), who argue that it is important to consider the peritraumatic defense processes that are activated and that subgroups of patients can be identified that are at different positions in the defense cascade. They specifically emphasize that memory processes are substantially different if persons are putting on a peritraumatic sympathetic activation versus those who went down the entire defense cascade where a parasympathetic dominance and dissociation prevail. This depends on the type of trauma and possibilities of the organism for optimal response. Extinction learning is especially hampered by dissociative\",\"PeriodicalId\":47289,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology\",\"volume\":\"217 1\",\"pages\":\"61-63\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2010-01-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"13\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409/A000012\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409/A000012","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13

摘要

在本期的《心理学杂志》中,我们汇集了一些关于创伤和记忆关系的文章和评论,重点是记忆机制在创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)中的作用。创伤性事件严重改变记忆的假设是一致的,但记忆缺陷也可能导致压力相关疾病的发展。尽管人们一致认为,在PTSD中,记忆机制是该疾病的核心,但对于病理的性质以及诸如对缺陷的评估等附加因素在多大程度上是重要的,人们存在分歧。正如Ehlers(2010)所指出的,有明确的证据表明,情绪记忆的优势往往是感官的,快速的,与创伤经历的非常令人厌恶的时刻有关,被视为即时而不是过去,并与非陈述性学习过程(如巴甫洛夫条件反射和启动)有关。虽然这些记忆似乎占主导地位,普遍化(见Lissek & Grillon, 2010),并且无法熄灭(Wessa & Flor, 2007),但创伤和生活的情景记忆似乎总体上受到影响,可能是碎片化(Brewin, 2007),缺乏联系(Michael, Ehlers, Halligan, & Clark, 2005)或过度普遍化(Schonfeld, Ehlers, Bollinghaus, & Rief, 2007)。早期关于创伤后应激障碍记忆机制的理论认为,记录在案的创伤后应激障碍海马体积的减少可能导致陈述性记忆和非陈述性记忆的分离(Elzinga & Bremner, 2002)。然而,这些改变并不像看起来那么简单,因为有创伤后应激障碍的人和没有创伤后应激障碍的人对创伤相关材料的回忆是相似的(Wessa, Jatzko, & Flor, 2006)。Koessler等(2010)的研究表明,PTSD与非PTSD受试者的检索诱发遗忘也没有显著差异。Diener, Flor和Wessa(2010)证明了与过度觉醒症状相关的情景记忆编码而非检索的特定缺陷。这表明需要对记忆机制进行非常彻底的分析,并且需要进一步区分诸如陈述性/非陈述性或情景性与语义性之类的类别,并且需要考虑这些记忆过程的具体方面-例如细化,连通性或创伤相关性。例如,海马体体积的减少也可能导致情境条件反射的减少,情境条件反射也是情感联想学习的一种形式,而更依赖于杏仁核的线索条件反射似乎得到了增强。我们(Lang et al., 2009)和其他人(Alvarez, Biggs, Chen, Pine, & Grillon, 2008;Marschner, Kalisch, Vervliet, Vansteenwegen, & Buchel, 2008)已经表明,海马体参与人类的情境条件反射,我们有初步数据表明,海马体体积小的人的情境条件反射受损(Pohlack, Liebscher, Ridder, Lipinski, & Flor, 2009)。因此,对创伤后应激障碍患者情境记忆的详细检查似乎是有必要的。环境对灭绝学习也很重要,生物体必须学会将条件刺激与特定的环境联系起来,不再与危险联系起来。Bouton及其同事(2006)和其他人强调了环境对灭绝的重要作用。因此,紊乱的情境条件反射可能不仅有助于PTSD患者的“现在感”和无法感到安全(因为安全和危险的情境无法正确区分),而且还可能损害线索条件恐惧反应的消退,因为消退情境与线索没有关联。这一推理与Schauer和Elbert(2010)的建议是一致的,他们认为重要的是要考虑被激活的创伤周围防御过程,并且可以识别出在防御级联中处于不同位置的患者亚群。他们特别强调,创伤周围交感神经激活的人与整个防御级联的人的记忆过程有本质上的不同副交感神经主导和分离盛行。这取决于创伤的类型和机体最佳反应的可能性。解离尤其阻碍了灭绝学习
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Memory and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
In this issue of the Zeitschrift fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology we have assembled a number of articles and comments that deal with the relationship of trauma and memory, with an emphasis on the role of memory mechanisms in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). There is uniformity in the assumption that memory is severely altered by traumatic events, but memory deficits may also contribute to the development of stress-related disorders. Although there is agreement that in PTSD memory mechanisms are at the core of the disorder, there is disagreement on the nature of the pathology and to what extent additional factors such as appraisal of the deficit are of importance. As Ehlers (2010) notes, there is clear evidence of a preponderance of emotional memories that are often sensory, fast, tied to very aversive moments of the traumatic experience, experienced as immediate rather than past, and related to non-declarative learning processes such as Pavlovian conditioning and priming. Whereas these memories seem to predominate, generalize (see Lissek & Grillon, 2010), and fail to extinguish (Wessa & Flor, 2007), episodic memories of the trauma and life in general seem to be affected, be it by fragmentation (Brewin, 2007), lack of connectedness (Michael, Ehlers, Halligan, & Clark, 2005), or overgeneralization (Schonfeld, Ehlers, Bollinghaus, & Rief, 2007). Early theories about memory mechanisms in PTSD suggested that the documented reduction of hippocampal volume in PTSD might lead to a dissociation of declarative and non-declarative memories (Elzinga & Bremner, 2002). However, these alterations are not as straightforward as it might seem since it could be shown that recall of trauma-related materials is similar in traumatized persons with and without PTSD (Wessa, Jatzko, & Flor, 2006). Retrieval-induced forgetting is also not significantly different in PTSD versus non-PTSD subjects as shown by Koessler and colleagues (2010). Diener, Flor, and Wessa (2010) demonstrate a specific deficit in encoding but not retrieval of episodic memory that is associated with hyperarousal symptoms. This suggests that a very thorough analysis of memory mechanisms is needed and that categories such as declarative/non-declarative or episodic versus semantic need to be further differentiated and specific aspects of these memory processes – such as elaboration, connectedness, or trauma-relevance – need to be considered. For example, the reduced hippocampal volumes could also lead to reduced context conditioning, which is also a form of emotional associative learning, whereas cue conditioning, which is more amygdala-dependent, seems to be enhanced. We (Lang et al., 2009) and others (Alvarez, Biggs, Chen, Pine, & Grillon, 2008; Marschner, Kalisch, Vervliet, Vansteenwegen, & Buchel, 2008) have shown that the hippocampus is involved in context conditioning in human beings and we have preliminary data that indicate impaired context conditioning in persons with small hippocampal volumes (Pohlack, Liebscher, Ridder, Lipinski, & Flor, 2009). A detailed examination of contextual memory in PTSD seems therefore warranted. Context is also important for extinction learning where the organism has to learn to associate the conditioned stimuli with a certain context, no longer with a danger. Bouton and colleagues (2006) and others have emphasized the important role of context for extinction. Disordered context conditioning might therefore not only contribute to the feeling of ‘‘nowness’’ that characterizes the intrusions of PTSD patients and to the inability to feel safe (since safe and dangerous contexts cannot be properly differentiated) but may also impair the extinction of the cue-conditioned fear response since the extinction context has not been associated to the cues. This reasoning is in line with the proposal by Schauer and Elbert (2010), who argue that it is important to consider the peritraumatic defense processes that are activated and that subgroups of patients can be identified that are at different positions in the defense cascade. They specifically emphasize that memory processes are substantially different if persons are putting on a peritraumatic sympathetic activation versus those who went down the entire defense cascade where a parasympathetic dominance and dissociation prevail. This depends on the type of trauma and possibilities of the organism for optimal response. Extinction learning is especially hampered by dissociative
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology
Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY-
CiteScore
4.10
自引率
5.60%
发文量
37
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信