利用节奏来促进水平跳跃的表现:个体化干预需求的范例

IF 0.6 4区 心理学 Q4 HOSPITALITY, LEISURE, SPORT & TOURISM
A. Macpherson, D. Collins, P. Graham-Smith, A. Turner
{"title":"利用节奏来促进水平跳跃的表现:个体化干预需求的范例","authors":"A. Macpherson, D. Collins, P. Graham-Smith, A. Turner","doi":"10.7352/IJSP.2013.44.093","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"1 The current study compared and contrasted the optimal regulation of stride patterns in the 2 horizontal jumping events for 6 British athletes of international standard. Long jump and 3 triple jump approach data were collected over a 3-year period in international and domestic 4 competitions and considered against the distances achieved. Results suggest that on approach 5 to the take-off board, the majority of athletes’ jumps of greater length (intra-athlete) are 6 associated with a low variability, rhythmical footfall. Given the variable approach strategies 7 used by the athletes in question, and consequently the theoretical implications the data set 8 holds, tentative conclusions are drawn regarding the means by which scientists and coaches 9 should assess and design suitable performance focused interventions for elite performers 10 based on individual responses. 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 Horizontal Jump Performance Using Rhythmicity to Promote Performance in Horizontal Jumps: An Exemplar of the Need 1 for Individually-Tailored Interventions 2 Determining how skilled performers execute goal-directed behaviours and the means 3 that enable performance to be enhanced is an essential role which applied sport psychologists 4 often conduct in partnership with other scientists. In doing so, scientist-practitioners often 5 seek guidance from the prevailing theoretical and/or empirical paradigms before applying this 6 knowledge to the practical problem at hand. However, this may sometimes take thinking in 7 an erroneous or less than optimum direction, especially in the special cases of elite 8 performers. In this regard, recent evidence suggests that there are significant advantages to 9 examining how skilled performers organise movement patterns on an intra-individual basis, 10 despite commonalities in patterns of co-ordination being evident in participants of similar 11 ability. For example, Chow, Davids, Button, and Koh, (2006) stated that if skilled 12 participants are grouped together for the purposes of movement analysis, effects of interest, 13 such as control strategies may become masked. 14 Support for pursuing applied sport psychology on a case-by-case basis is illustrated by 15 an increasing number of studies that demonstrated individual-specific findings with reference 16 to performers’ coordination, their responses to appropriate sources of information for 17 instruction and adaptation to physical stress. For example, Beavan, Gill, and Cook (2008a) 18 demonstrated that professional rugby union players with broadly similar training backgrounds 19 responded to group-prescribed resistance training through individual hormonal responses. It 20 was determined that certain players responded positively to some training stresses, but not to 21 other forms of resistance training. In a 3-week cross-over design, the same players alternated 22 between sessions that produced high and low levels of testosterone. It was found that 23 favoured exercise selection elicited players’ maximum testosterone response and resulted in a 24 statistically significant strength gain. However, when the less favoured protocol was used, 25 Horizontal Jump Performance there was either no change, or a significant decline in tested strength (Beavan, Gill, & Cook, 1 2008b). Such individualised hormonal responses to training adaptations in professional sport 2 lend credence to the claim that advanced performers should be investigated on an individual 3 basis. 4 Further support for this contention is provided by examples of empirical research that 5 advocated the application of generic instructional principles to elite groups with—in some 6 cases—less than optimum results. Research carried out with shooters (e.g., rifle, pistol, and 7 archery) by Helin, Sihvonen, and Hanninen, (1987) and Landers, Christina, Hatfield, Daniels 8 and Doyle (1980) suggested that shooters performed to an optimised level when they shot in9 between heartbeats when the cardiac cycle is in diastole, or in what is termed the inter-beat 10 interval (IBI). However, analysis of case study data of six elite-level shooters conducted by 11 Bellamy, Collins, Holmes and Loze (1999) indicated that there is insufficient evidence to 12 advocate a universal strategy of shooting during diastole in elite shooters, since four of the 13 six shooters examined actually shot on the beat. Rather, they suggested the essential need for 14 checks that could detect individual patterns associated with better performance. Whilst this 15 example is somewhat dated, it is uncertain whether the application of generic instructions to 16 elite or advanced performers, albeit derived from peer-reviewed empirical work would 17 automatically result in an improvement to personal performance. 18 A more recent example of the contrasting instructional content of pre-performance 19 routines (PPRs) among elite golfers (i.e., mean handicap of +1.5) was uncovered by 20 Cotterrill, Sanders, and Collins (2010). Whilst the content and firing modality of PPRs has 21 been well researched in relation to self-paced skills (Singer, 2002), research conducted by 22 Cotterrill et al. (2010) showed that the development of a PPR for an elite golfer is 23 idiosyncratic; contingent upon their coping resources; factors that constitute their 24 personalities; and a tendency to appraise the context of each golf shot prior to task execution. 25 Horizontal Jump Performance Accordingly, we suggest that working uncritically from theory to intervention is not an 1 appropriate strategy for groups of advanced learners. 2 In light of these two examples (shooting & golf), it is important to reflect upon the 3 work of Newell, Liu, and Mayer-Kress (2005). Specifically, they proposed that different 4 types of information are differentially effective for athletes, and that the efficacy of 5 alternative sources of information is dependent on the task and the skill level of the learner. 6 Consequently, it is unlikely that an athlete’s potential will be maximised if a ‘one instruction 7 fits all’ approach is used. Therefore, the onus is on applied scientists to determine the most 8 effective sources of information to communicate with and inform athletes’ motor systems. 9 For example, in a study that provided concurrent auditory feedback to gymnasts on the 10 pommel horse, body segment alignment improved by 2.3% between the experimental and 11 control groups. Researchers concluded that auditory feedback provided in real-time could be 12 used to correct complex movements (Baudry, Leroy, Thouvarecq, & Chollet, 2006) as 13 opposed to the more commonly applied video techniques. Interestingly, anecdotal evidence 14 suggested that experienced horizontal jumps coaches could listen to the footfall of their 15 athletes, whilst looking in the opposite direction, and were able to confidently assert whether 16 their athlete had jumped well, or not (Moore, 2006). 17 Whilst it seems that some horizontal jumps coaches have become aware of, and 18 utilise, the rhythm engendered by the auditory output of an athlete’s footfall signature on 19 approach to the take-off board, there is interesting empirical research that has demonstrated 20 the beneficial effect of utilising an auditory output to augment visual information. In a 21 complex series of five experiments Vroomen and de Gelder (2000) demonstrated that 22 perceptual organisation in the auditory modality impacted upon perceptual accuracy in the 23 visual modality. Specifically, a high tone embedded in a series of low tones improved 24 detection of a visual target, when the visual target was presented at the same time – provided 25 Horizontal Jump Performance that the tone was abrupt and distinct from background noise or contaminating melodies. 1 These studies demonstrated that auditory stimuli can be used to enhance the detection of 2 visual information. Therefore, it is possible that a holistic rhythm of an athlete’s footfall prior 3 to contact with the take-off board may serve as an aid to improve foot-to-board accuracy – 4 provided an athlete’s footfall patterns are deemed to be relatively stable. 5 Whilst some athletes might not find this modality of movement correction useful, data 6 presented in this paper provide an opportunity for scientists, applied practitioners, and 7 coaches to consider the implications of working with elite performers through an exemplar of 8 this ‘individualised’ approach. Specifically, the applicability of prevailing theoretical 9 paradigms pertaining to the approach and preparation phases of horizontal jumps is critiqued 10 through consideration of data obtained from a cohort of elite performers. 11 Horizontal Jumps: Event demands 12 For athletes participating in horizontal jumps (HJ) the apparent difficulty associated 13 with executing the task lies in direct contrast to the simplicity of the objective. In particular, 14 the challenge rests in the trade-off between maintaining peak horizontal velocity (athletes 15 typically achieve peak horizontal velocities at take-off of 9.4 m/s for men, and 8.6 m/s for 16 women; see Linthorne, 2007) and being accurate on to the take-off board. To maximise the 17 measured distance, the space between the jump take-off and the edge of the take-off board 18 must be minimised. In addition, the athlete is required to create good ‘lift’ at take-off, 19 generating vertical impulse by accelerating body parts upwards during the final contact with 20 the board without employing large braking forces. A key factor in generating vertical lift is 21 the transference of horizontal velocity accrued on the run-up. Therefore, accuracy onto the 22 take-off board, at speed, is critical to eventual distance achieved (Hay, Miller, & Canterna, 23 1986). 24 Horizontal Jump Performance At an elite level the difficulty of “hitting the board” in HJ is exemplified by 1 1 participant in this investigation, an elite horizontal jumper, who had 10 years of ","PeriodicalId":54940,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Sport Psychology","volume":"44 1","pages":"93-110"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6000,"publicationDate":"2013-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"7","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Using rhythmicity to promote performance in horizontal jumps: An exemplar of the need for individually-tailored interventions\",\"authors\":\"A. Macpherson, D. Collins, P. Graham-Smith, A. Turner\",\"doi\":\"10.7352/IJSP.2013.44.093\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"1 The current study compared and contrasted the optimal regulation of stride patterns in the 2 horizontal jumping events for 6 British athletes of international standard. Long jump and 3 triple jump approach data were collected over a 3-year period in international and domestic 4 competitions and considered against the distances achieved. Results suggest that on approach 5 to the take-off board, the majority of athletes’ jumps of greater length (intra-athlete) are 6 associated with a low variability, rhythmical footfall. Given the variable approach strategies 7 used by the athletes in question, and consequently the theoretical implications the data set 8 holds, tentative conclusions are drawn regarding the means by which scientists and coaches 9 should assess and design suitable performance focused interventions for elite performers 10 based on individual responses. 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 Horizontal Jump Performance Using Rhythmicity to Promote Performance in Horizontal Jumps: An Exemplar of the Need 1 for Individually-Tailored Interventions 2 Determining how skilled performers execute goal-directed behaviours and the means 3 that enable performance to be enhanced is an essential role which applied sport psychologists 4 often conduct in partnership with other scientists. In doing so, scientist-practitioners often 5 seek guidance from the prevailing theoretical and/or empirical paradigms before applying this 6 knowledge to the practical problem at hand. However, this may sometimes take thinking in 7 an erroneous or less than optimum direction, especially in the special cases of elite 8 performers. In this regard, recent evidence suggests that there are significant advantages to 9 examining how skilled performers organise movement patterns on an intra-individual basis, 10 despite commonalities in patterns of co-ordination being evident in participants of similar 11 ability. For example, Chow, Davids, Button, and Koh, (2006) stated that if skilled 12 participants are grouped together for the purposes of movement analysis, effects of interest, 13 such as control strategies may become masked. 14 Support for pursuing applied sport psychology on a case-by-case basis is illustrated by 15 an increasing number of studies that demonstrated individual-specific findings with reference 16 to performers’ coordination, their responses to appropriate sources of information for 17 instruction and adaptation to physical stress. For example, Beavan, Gill, and Cook (2008a) 18 demonstrated that professional rugby union players with broadly similar training backgrounds 19 responded to group-prescribed resistance training through individual hormonal responses. It 20 was determined that certain players responded positively to some training stresses, but not to 21 other forms of resistance training. In a 3-week cross-over design, the same players alternated 22 between sessions that produced high and low levels of testosterone. It was found that 23 favoured exercise selection elicited players’ maximum testosterone response and resulted in a 24 statistically significant strength gain. However, when the less favoured protocol was used, 25 Horizontal Jump Performance there was either no change, or a significant decline in tested strength (Beavan, Gill, & Cook, 1 2008b). Such individualised hormonal responses to training adaptations in professional sport 2 lend credence to the claim that advanced performers should be investigated on an individual 3 basis. 4 Further support for this contention is provided by examples of empirical research that 5 advocated the application of generic instructional principles to elite groups with—in some 6 cases—less than optimum results. Research carried out with shooters (e.g., rifle, pistol, and 7 archery) by Helin, Sihvonen, and Hanninen, (1987) and Landers, Christina, Hatfield, Daniels 8 and Doyle (1980) suggested that shooters performed to an optimised level when they shot in9 between heartbeats when the cardiac cycle is in diastole, or in what is termed the inter-beat 10 interval (IBI). However, analysis of case study data of six elite-level shooters conducted by 11 Bellamy, Collins, Holmes and Loze (1999) indicated that there is insufficient evidence to 12 advocate a universal strategy of shooting during diastole in elite shooters, since four of the 13 six shooters examined actually shot on the beat. Rather, they suggested the essential need for 14 checks that could detect individual patterns associated with better performance. Whilst this 15 example is somewhat dated, it is uncertain whether the application of generic instructions to 16 elite or advanced performers, albeit derived from peer-reviewed empirical work would 17 automatically result in an improvement to personal performance. 18 A more recent example of the contrasting instructional content of pre-performance 19 routines (PPRs) among elite golfers (i.e., mean handicap of +1.5) was uncovered by 20 Cotterrill, Sanders, and Collins (2010). Whilst the content and firing modality of PPRs has 21 been well researched in relation to self-paced skills (Singer, 2002), research conducted by 22 Cotterrill et al. (2010) showed that the development of a PPR for an elite golfer is 23 idiosyncratic; contingent upon their coping resources; factors that constitute their 24 personalities; and a tendency to appraise the context of each golf shot prior to task execution. 25 Horizontal Jump Performance Accordingly, we suggest that working uncritically from theory to intervention is not an 1 appropriate strategy for groups of advanced learners. 2 In light of these two examples (shooting & golf), it is important to reflect upon the 3 work of Newell, Liu, and Mayer-Kress (2005). Specifically, they proposed that different 4 types of information are differentially effective for athletes, and that the efficacy of 5 alternative sources of information is dependent on the task and the skill level of the learner. 6 Consequently, it is unlikely that an athlete’s potential will be maximised if a ‘one instruction 7 fits all’ approach is used. Therefore, the onus is on applied scientists to determine the most 8 effective sources of information to communicate with and inform athletes’ motor systems. 9 For example, in a study that provided concurrent auditory feedback to gymnasts on the 10 pommel horse, body segment alignment improved by 2.3% between the experimental and 11 control groups. Researchers concluded that auditory feedback provided in real-time could be 12 used to correct complex movements (Baudry, Leroy, Thouvarecq, & Chollet, 2006) as 13 opposed to the more commonly applied video techniques. Interestingly, anecdotal evidence 14 suggested that experienced horizontal jumps coaches could listen to the footfall of their 15 athletes, whilst looking in the opposite direction, and were able to confidently assert whether 16 their athlete had jumped well, or not (Moore, 2006). 17 Whilst it seems that some horizontal jumps coaches have become aware of, and 18 utilise, the rhythm engendered by the auditory output of an athlete’s footfall signature on 19 approach to the take-off board, there is interesting empirical research that has demonstrated 20 the beneficial effect of utilising an auditory output to augment visual information. In a 21 complex series of five experiments Vroomen and de Gelder (2000) demonstrated that 22 perceptual organisation in the auditory modality impacted upon perceptual accuracy in the 23 visual modality. Specifically, a high tone embedded in a series of low tones improved 24 detection of a visual target, when the visual target was presented at the same time – provided 25 Horizontal Jump Performance that the tone was abrupt and distinct from background noise or contaminating melodies. 1 These studies demonstrated that auditory stimuli can be used to enhance the detection of 2 visual information. Therefore, it is possible that a holistic rhythm of an athlete’s footfall prior 3 to contact with the take-off board may serve as an aid to improve foot-to-board accuracy – 4 provided an athlete’s footfall patterns are deemed to be relatively stable. 5 Whilst some athletes might not find this modality of movement correction useful, data 6 presented in this paper provide an opportunity for scientists, applied practitioners, and 7 coaches to consider the implications of working with elite performers through an exemplar of 8 this ‘individualised’ approach. Specifically, the applicability of prevailing theoretical 9 paradigms pertaining to the approach and preparation phases of horizontal jumps is critiqued 10 through consideration of data obtained from a cohort of elite performers. 11 Horizontal Jumps: Event demands 12 For athletes participating in horizontal jumps (HJ) the apparent difficulty associated 13 with executing the task lies in direct contrast to the simplicity of the objective. In particular, 14 the challenge rests in the trade-off between maintaining peak horizontal velocity (athletes 15 typically achieve peak horizontal velocities at take-off of 9.4 m/s for men, and 8.6 m/s for 16 women; see Linthorne, 2007) and being accurate on to the take-off board. To maximise the 17 measured distance, the space between the jump take-off and the edge of the take-off board 18 must be minimised. In addition, the athlete is required to create good ‘lift’ at take-off, 19 generating vertical impulse by accelerating body parts upwards during the final contact with 20 the board without employing large braking forces. A key factor in generating vertical lift is 21 the transference of horizontal velocity accrued on the run-up. Therefore, accuracy onto the 22 take-off board, at speed, is critical to eventual distance achieved (Hay, Miller, & Canterna, 23 1986). 24 Horizontal Jump Performance At an elite level the difficulty of “hitting the board” in HJ is exemplified by 1 1 participant in this investigation, an elite horizontal jumper, who had 10 years of \",\"PeriodicalId\":54940,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"International Journal of Sport Psychology\",\"volume\":\"44 1\",\"pages\":\"93-110\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.6000,\"publicationDate\":\"2013-03-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"7\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"International Journal of Sport Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"3\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP.2013.44.093\",\"RegionNum\":4,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q4\",\"JCRName\":\"HOSPITALITY, LEISURE, SPORT & TOURISM\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"International Journal of Sport Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP.2013.44.093","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"HOSPITALITY, LEISURE, SPORT & TOURISM","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7

摘要

1本研究对6名具有国际水平的英国运动员在2个水平跳远项目中跨步模式的最佳调节进行了比较和对比。本研究收集了3年来在国际和国内4项比赛中跳远和三级跳远方法的数据,并将其与取得的距离进行比较。结果表明,在接近起跳板的第5步,大多数运动员的较长跳跃(运动员内部)与低变异性、有节奏的步法有关。考虑到所讨论的运动员使用的可变方法策略,以及数据集所具有的理论含义,我们得出了关于科学家和教练应该根据个人反应评估和设计适合精英运动员的专注于表现的干预手段的初步结论。运用节奏性促进水平跳跃表现:个性化干预需求的范例确定熟练的表演者如何执行目标导向的行为以及提高表现的手段是应用运动心理学家经常与其他科学家合作所扮演的重要角色。在这样做的过程中,科学家实践者在将这些知识应用于手头的实际问题之前,通常会从流行的理论和/或经验范式中寻求指导。然而,这有时可能会导致思考方向错误或不太理想,尤其是在精英人士的特殊情况下。在这方面,最近的证据表明,尽管能力相似的参与者在协调模式上存在明显的共性,但研究熟练的表演者如何在个体内部组织动作模式具有显著的优势。例如,Chow, david, Button和Koh(2006)指出,如果熟练的参与者为了动作分析的目的而被分组在一起,兴趣的影响,如控制策略,可能会被掩盖。越来越多的研究表明,在表演者的协调、他们对适当的指导信息来源的反应和对身体压力的适应方面,有个别的发现。这些研究表明,在个案基础上追求应用运动心理学是得到支持的。例如,Beavan, Gill和Cook (2008a) 18证明了训练背景大致相似的职业橄榄球联盟球员19通过个体激素反应对团体规定的抗阻训练做出反应。研究确定,某些运动员对某些训练压力反应积极,但对其他形式的阻力训练则不然。在为期3周的交叉设计中,相同的玩家在产生高水平和低水平睾丸激素的会话之间交替进行。结果发现,23种有利的运动选择引起了运动员最大的睾酮反应,并导致24种有统计学意义的力量增加。然而,当使用不太受欢迎的方案时,25水平跳跃性能要么没有变化,要么测试强度显著下降(Beavan, Gill, & Cook, 1 2008b)。在职业体育运动中,这种针对训练适应的个体化荷尔蒙反应,为“高水平运动员应该以个人为基础进行调查”的说法提供了证据。实证研究的例子为这一论点提供了进一步的支持,这些研究提倡将通用教学原则应用于精英群体,在某些情况下,结果并不理想。Helin、Sihvonen和Hanninen(1987)以及Landers、Christina、Hatfield、Daniels和Doyle(1980)对射击运动员(如步枪、手枪和射箭运动员)进行的研究表明,当他们在心跳周期处于舒张期或所谓的搏动间隔(IBI)时进行射击时,射击运动员的表现达到了最佳水平。然而,Bellamy, Collins, Holmes和Loze(1999)对6名精英级射击运动员的案例研究数据进行的分析表明,没有足够的证据支持精英级射击运动员在舒张期射击的普遍策略,因为13名被调查的6名射击运动员中有4名实际上是在拍时射击的。相反,他们建议有必要进行14项检查,这些检查可以检测出与更好的表现相关的单个模式。虽然这个例子有些过时,但不确定的是,将通用指令应用于精英或高级执行者(尽管来自同行评议的实证研究)是否会自动导致个人绩效的提高。最近的一个例子是,在精英高尔夫球手(即平均差点为+1)中,表演前套路(PPRs)的教学内容对比。 Cotterrill, Sanders, and Collins(2010)发现了5)。虽然PPR的内容和射击方式已经被很好地研究了与自我节奏技能的关系(Singer, 2002),但Cotterrill等人(2010)进行的研究表明,精英高尔夫球手的PPR发展是特殊的;取决于他们的应对资源;构成他们24种人格的因素;以及在执行任务之前评估每次高尔夫击球的背景的倾向。因此,我们建议不加批判地从理论到干预并不是一种适合高级学习者群体的策略。2根据这两个例子(射击和高尔夫),反思Newell, Liu, and Mayer-Kress(2005)的3个作品是很重要的。具体来说,他们提出了不同类型的信息对运动员的有效性是不同的,并且5种替代信息来源的有效性取决于学习者的任务和技能水平。因此,如果采用“一刀切”的方法,运动员的潜力不太可能得到最大限度的发挥。因此,应用科学家的责任是确定最有效的信息来源,与运动员的运动系统进行沟通。例如,在一项研究中,在鞍马上给体操运动员提供同步的听觉反馈,在实验组和11个对照组之间,身体部位的对齐度提高了2.3%。研究人员得出结论,实时提供的听觉反馈可以用来纠正复杂的动作(Baudry, Leroy, Thouvarecq, & Chollet, 2006),而不是更常用的视频技术。有趣的是,坊间证据表明,经验丰富的水平跳远教练可以一边听着运动员的脚步声,一边看向相反的方向,并能够自信地断言他们的运动员是否跳得好(Moore, 2006)。虽然一些水平跳跃教练似乎已经意识到并利用了运动员在接近起跳板时脚法特征的听觉输出所产生的节奏,但有一项有趣的实证研究已经证明了利用听觉输出来增强视觉信息的有益效果。Vroomen和de Gelder(2000)在21个复杂系列的5个实验中证明,听觉模态中的22个知觉组织影响了23个视觉模态中的知觉准确性。具体来说,当视觉目标同时出现时,嵌入在一系列低音调中的高音提高了对视觉目标的检测,提供了25水平跳跃性能,音调是突然的,与背景噪音或污染旋律不同。这些研究表明,听觉刺激可以用来增强对视觉信息的检测。因此,如果运动员的脚法模式被认为是相对稳定的,那么在接触起跳板之前,运动员脚法的整体节奏可能有助于提高脚法对跳板的准确性。虽然有些运动员可能不会发现这种运动纠正方式有用,但本文提供的数据为科学家、应用从业者和教练提供了一个机会,让他们通过这种“个性化”方法的范例来考虑与精英运动员合作的影响。具体来说,通过考虑从一群优秀运动员那里获得的数据,对有关水平跳跃的方法和准备阶段的主流理论范式的适用性进行了批评。水平跳跃:项目要求对于参加水平跳跃的运动员来说,执行任务的明显难度与目标的简单性形成了直接的对比。特别是,挑战在于保持峰值水平速度之间的权衡(运动员通常在起飞时达到9.4米/秒的峰值水平速度,而女子为8.6米/秒;参见Linthorne, 2007)和准确的起飞板。为了使所测距离最大化,起跳点与起跳板边缘之间的距离必须最小化。此外,运动员需要在起飞时创造良好的“升力”,在与跳板的最后接触中,在不使用大制动力的情况下,通过加速身体部位向上产生垂直冲力。产生垂直升力的一个关键因素是在助跑过程中积累的水平速度的传递。因此,在22起飞板上的准确性,在速度上,对最终达到的距离至关重要(Hay, Miller, & Canterna, 23 1986)。 在HJ的精英水平上,“落地”的难度以11名参与者为例,他们是一名优秀的水平跳远运动员,有10年的训练经验
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Using rhythmicity to promote performance in horizontal jumps: An exemplar of the need for individually-tailored interventions
1 The current study compared and contrasted the optimal regulation of stride patterns in the 2 horizontal jumping events for 6 British athletes of international standard. Long jump and 3 triple jump approach data were collected over a 3-year period in international and domestic 4 competitions and considered against the distances achieved. Results suggest that on approach 5 to the take-off board, the majority of athletes’ jumps of greater length (intra-athlete) are 6 associated with a low variability, rhythmical footfall. Given the variable approach strategies 7 used by the athletes in question, and consequently the theoretical implications the data set 8 holds, tentative conclusions are drawn regarding the means by which scientists and coaches 9 should assess and design suitable performance focused interventions for elite performers 10 based on individual responses. 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 Horizontal Jump Performance Using Rhythmicity to Promote Performance in Horizontal Jumps: An Exemplar of the Need 1 for Individually-Tailored Interventions 2 Determining how skilled performers execute goal-directed behaviours and the means 3 that enable performance to be enhanced is an essential role which applied sport psychologists 4 often conduct in partnership with other scientists. In doing so, scientist-practitioners often 5 seek guidance from the prevailing theoretical and/or empirical paradigms before applying this 6 knowledge to the practical problem at hand. However, this may sometimes take thinking in 7 an erroneous or less than optimum direction, especially in the special cases of elite 8 performers. In this regard, recent evidence suggests that there are significant advantages to 9 examining how skilled performers organise movement patterns on an intra-individual basis, 10 despite commonalities in patterns of co-ordination being evident in participants of similar 11 ability. For example, Chow, Davids, Button, and Koh, (2006) stated that if skilled 12 participants are grouped together for the purposes of movement analysis, effects of interest, 13 such as control strategies may become masked. 14 Support for pursuing applied sport psychology on a case-by-case basis is illustrated by 15 an increasing number of studies that demonstrated individual-specific findings with reference 16 to performers’ coordination, their responses to appropriate sources of information for 17 instruction and adaptation to physical stress. For example, Beavan, Gill, and Cook (2008a) 18 demonstrated that professional rugby union players with broadly similar training backgrounds 19 responded to group-prescribed resistance training through individual hormonal responses. It 20 was determined that certain players responded positively to some training stresses, but not to 21 other forms of resistance training. In a 3-week cross-over design, the same players alternated 22 between sessions that produced high and low levels of testosterone. It was found that 23 favoured exercise selection elicited players’ maximum testosterone response and resulted in a 24 statistically significant strength gain. However, when the less favoured protocol was used, 25 Horizontal Jump Performance there was either no change, or a significant decline in tested strength (Beavan, Gill, & Cook, 1 2008b). Such individualised hormonal responses to training adaptations in professional sport 2 lend credence to the claim that advanced performers should be investigated on an individual 3 basis. 4 Further support for this contention is provided by examples of empirical research that 5 advocated the application of generic instructional principles to elite groups with—in some 6 cases—less than optimum results. Research carried out with shooters (e.g., rifle, pistol, and 7 archery) by Helin, Sihvonen, and Hanninen, (1987) and Landers, Christina, Hatfield, Daniels 8 and Doyle (1980) suggested that shooters performed to an optimised level when they shot in9 between heartbeats when the cardiac cycle is in diastole, or in what is termed the inter-beat 10 interval (IBI). However, analysis of case study data of six elite-level shooters conducted by 11 Bellamy, Collins, Holmes and Loze (1999) indicated that there is insufficient evidence to 12 advocate a universal strategy of shooting during diastole in elite shooters, since four of the 13 six shooters examined actually shot on the beat. Rather, they suggested the essential need for 14 checks that could detect individual patterns associated with better performance. Whilst this 15 example is somewhat dated, it is uncertain whether the application of generic instructions to 16 elite or advanced performers, albeit derived from peer-reviewed empirical work would 17 automatically result in an improvement to personal performance. 18 A more recent example of the contrasting instructional content of pre-performance 19 routines (PPRs) among elite golfers (i.e., mean handicap of +1.5) was uncovered by 20 Cotterrill, Sanders, and Collins (2010). Whilst the content and firing modality of PPRs has 21 been well researched in relation to self-paced skills (Singer, 2002), research conducted by 22 Cotterrill et al. (2010) showed that the development of a PPR for an elite golfer is 23 idiosyncratic; contingent upon their coping resources; factors that constitute their 24 personalities; and a tendency to appraise the context of each golf shot prior to task execution. 25 Horizontal Jump Performance Accordingly, we suggest that working uncritically from theory to intervention is not an 1 appropriate strategy for groups of advanced learners. 2 In light of these two examples (shooting & golf), it is important to reflect upon the 3 work of Newell, Liu, and Mayer-Kress (2005). Specifically, they proposed that different 4 types of information are differentially effective for athletes, and that the efficacy of 5 alternative sources of information is dependent on the task and the skill level of the learner. 6 Consequently, it is unlikely that an athlete’s potential will be maximised if a ‘one instruction 7 fits all’ approach is used. Therefore, the onus is on applied scientists to determine the most 8 effective sources of information to communicate with and inform athletes’ motor systems. 9 For example, in a study that provided concurrent auditory feedback to gymnasts on the 10 pommel horse, body segment alignment improved by 2.3% between the experimental and 11 control groups. Researchers concluded that auditory feedback provided in real-time could be 12 used to correct complex movements (Baudry, Leroy, Thouvarecq, & Chollet, 2006) as 13 opposed to the more commonly applied video techniques. Interestingly, anecdotal evidence 14 suggested that experienced horizontal jumps coaches could listen to the footfall of their 15 athletes, whilst looking in the opposite direction, and were able to confidently assert whether 16 their athlete had jumped well, or not (Moore, 2006). 17 Whilst it seems that some horizontal jumps coaches have become aware of, and 18 utilise, the rhythm engendered by the auditory output of an athlete’s footfall signature on 19 approach to the take-off board, there is interesting empirical research that has demonstrated 20 the beneficial effect of utilising an auditory output to augment visual information. In a 21 complex series of five experiments Vroomen and de Gelder (2000) demonstrated that 22 perceptual organisation in the auditory modality impacted upon perceptual accuracy in the 23 visual modality. Specifically, a high tone embedded in a series of low tones improved 24 detection of a visual target, when the visual target was presented at the same time – provided 25 Horizontal Jump Performance that the tone was abrupt and distinct from background noise or contaminating melodies. 1 These studies demonstrated that auditory stimuli can be used to enhance the detection of 2 visual information. Therefore, it is possible that a holistic rhythm of an athlete’s footfall prior 3 to contact with the take-off board may serve as an aid to improve foot-to-board accuracy – 4 provided an athlete’s footfall patterns are deemed to be relatively stable. 5 Whilst some athletes might not find this modality of movement correction useful, data 6 presented in this paper provide an opportunity for scientists, applied practitioners, and 7 coaches to consider the implications of working with elite performers through an exemplar of 8 this ‘individualised’ approach. Specifically, the applicability of prevailing theoretical 9 paradigms pertaining to the approach and preparation phases of horizontal jumps is critiqued 10 through consideration of data obtained from a cohort of elite performers. 11 Horizontal Jumps: Event demands 12 For athletes participating in horizontal jumps (HJ) the apparent difficulty associated 13 with executing the task lies in direct contrast to the simplicity of the objective. In particular, 14 the challenge rests in the trade-off between maintaining peak horizontal velocity (athletes 15 typically achieve peak horizontal velocities at take-off of 9.4 m/s for men, and 8.6 m/s for 16 women; see Linthorne, 2007) and being accurate on to the take-off board. To maximise the 17 measured distance, the space between the jump take-off and the edge of the take-off board 18 must be minimised. In addition, the athlete is required to create good ‘lift’ at take-off, 19 generating vertical impulse by accelerating body parts upwards during the final contact with 20 the board without employing large braking forces. A key factor in generating vertical lift is 21 the transference of horizontal velocity accrued on the run-up. Therefore, accuracy onto the 22 take-off board, at speed, is critical to eventual distance achieved (Hay, Miller, & Canterna, 23 1986). 24 Horizontal Jump Performance At an elite level the difficulty of “hitting the board” in HJ is exemplified by 1 1 participant in this investigation, an elite horizontal jumper, who had 10 years of
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
12.50%
发文量
0
审稿时长
>12 weeks
期刊介绍: The International Journal of Sport Psychology publishes empirical and theoretical contributions in the human movement sciences from all over the world. Manuscripts related to psychology, sport pedagogy, exercise and sport performance are suited to the Journal''s scope. IJSP''s aims are to disseminate results of rigorous and relevant studies, to expose positions and commentaries regarding the development of theory and confirmation or contradiction of previous findings. IJSP entertains various methodologies encompassing coherence among epistemology, research questions, tools, statistical or clinical analyses and discussion or potential applications. Qualitative and quantitative analyses as well as case studies are of interest when appropriately used. IJSP is comprised of the following sections related to human movement sciences: -Motor learning and control -Cognition -Health and exercise -Social psychology -Intervention / Clinical / counseling psychology
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信