Alessandra Pecunioso, Maria Santacà, M. E. M. Petrazzini, C. Agrillo
{"title":"对视觉错觉的易感性与大脑的相对大小有关吗?来自小脑袋物种的见解","authors":"Alessandra Pecunioso, Maria Santacà, M. E. M. Petrazzini, C. Agrillo","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150003","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The study of visual illusions represents a useful tool in different research fields. Visual illusions are commonly used to establish the perceptual mechanisms underlying our perception of static and dynamic events (for a review, see Eagleman, 2001; Gregory, 1997). Visual illusions have become a valid tool in clinical psychology as a noninvasive screening for detecting schizophrenic and other psychopathological traits (e.g., Gori, Molteni, & Facoetti, 2016; Notredame, Pins, Deneve, & Jardri, 2014; Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Simon, Suganuma, & Tavares, 2008). Last, they represent a powerful set of stimuli to compare the vision of human and non human animals. With respect to this issue, the past two decades have been characterized by a growing number of studies finding that apes, old-world monkeys, and new-world monkeys are susceptible to many visual illusions. Nonhuman primates perceive size illusions (e.g., chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Parrish & Beran, 2014; capuchin monkeys, Sapajus apella: Parrish, Brosnan, & Beran, 2015; Suganuma, Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Castro, & Tavares, 2007), depth illusions (baboons, Papio papio: Barbet & Fagot, 2002), orientation illusions (rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta: Agrillo, Parrish, & Beran, 2014b), numerosity illusions (rhesus monkeys: Beran, 2006), and motion illusions (rhesus monkeys: Agrillo, Gori, & Beran, 2015).","PeriodicalId":0,"journal":{"name":"","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Is the susceptibility to visual illusions related to the relative brain size? Insights from small-brained species\",\"authors\":\"Alessandra Pecunioso, Maria Santacà, M. E. M. Petrazzini, C. Agrillo\",\"doi\":\"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150003\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"The study of visual illusions represents a useful tool in different research fields. Visual illusions are commonly used to establish the perceptual mechanisms underlying our perception of static and dynamic events (for a review, see Eagleman, 2001; Gregory, 1997). Visual illusions have become a valid tool in clinical psychology as a noninvasive screening for detecting schizophrenic and other psychopathological traits (e.g., Gori, Molteni, & Facoetti, 2016; Notredame, Pins, Deneve, & Jardri, 2014; Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Simon, Suganuma, & Tavares, 2008). Last, they represent a powerful set of stimuli to compare the vision of human and non human animals. With respect to this issue, the past two decades have been characterized by a growing number of studies finding that apes, old-world monkeys, and new-world monkeys are susceptible to many visual illusions. Nonhuman primates perceive size illusions (e.g., chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Parrish & Beran, 2014; capuchin monkeys, Sapajus apella: Parrish, Brosnan, & Beran, 2015; Suganuma, Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Castro, & Tavares, 2007), depth illusions (baboons, Papio papio: Barbet & Fagot, 2002), orientation illusions (rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta: Agrillo, Parrish, & Beran, 2014b), numerosity illusions (rhesus monkeys: Beran, 2006), and motion illusions (rhesus monkeys: Agrillo, Gori, & Beran, 2015).\",\"PeriodicalId\":0,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0,\"publicationDate\":\"2020-01-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150003\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150003","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
Is the susceptibility to visual illusions related to the relative brain size? Insights from small-brained species
The study of visual illusions represents a useful tool in different research fields. Visual illusions are commonly used to establish the perceptual mechanisms underlying our perception of static and dynamic events (for a review, see Eagleman, 2001; Gregory, 1997). Visual illusions have become a valid tool in clinical psychology as a noninvasive screening for detecting schizophrenic and other psychopathological traits (e.g., Gori, Molteni, & Facoetti, 2016; Notredame, Pins, Deneve, & Jardri, 2014; Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Simon, Suganuma, & Tavares, 2008). Last, they represent a powerful set of stimuli to compare the vision of human and non human animals. With respect to this issue, the past two decades have been characterized by a growing number of studies finding that apes, old-world monkeys, and new-world monkeys are susceptible to many visual illusions. Nonhuman primates perceive size illusions (e.g., chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Parrish & Beran, 2014; capuchin monkeys, Sapajus apella: Parrish, Brosnan, & Beran, 2015; Suganuma, Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Castro, & Tavares, 2007), depth illusions (baboons, Papio papio: Barbet & Fagot, 2002), orientation illusions (rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta: Agrillo, Parrish, & Beran, 2014b), numerosity illusions (rhesus monkeys: Beran, 2006), and motion illusions (rhesus monkeys: Agrillo, Gori, & Beran, 2015).