惊人的高薪:研究野生动物社会性的当代方法

IF 6.3 2区 环境科学与生态学 Q1 ECOLOGY
Thibaud Gruber, Erica van de Waal
{"title":"惊人的高薪:研究野生动物社会性的当代方法","authors":"Thibaud Gruber,&nbsp;Erica van de Waal","doi":"10.1111/2041-210X.14178","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>From the early Lascaux painters to British naturalists and to modern scientists worldwide, throughout our history, our species has always watched other animals in their natural environment. In doing so, we were able to get a glimpse of the social life of animals from a wide variety of taxa, and to attempt to make sense of it, for all kinds of purposes, be it hunting or scientific knowledge. Often, those various purposes lead to the same outcomes: taking notice of their patterns and habits or recording their communicative displays and making use of them. Observing animals is not an easy task, and making sense of their sociality even less so. While observing wild animals has remained the major channel through which we can make sense of their social lives, humans are additionally aided by an ever-increasing tool set to do so, fuelled by our ever-improving technology as well as our reliance upon it (Henrich, <span>2017</span>). Such technological advances can be seen both through the methods we use when collecting data in the field and the ones we use to analyse the product of our research. The latter can be as diverse as vocal or urine samples, records of distances or interactions between individuals, or choices in a field experiment, and the field is aided greatly by a constant effort in developing new technologies to analyse them.</p><p>Our Joint Special Feature in <i>Methods in Ecology and Evolution</i> and the <i>Journal of Animal Ecology</i> aims to showcase contemporary methods for studying sociality in the wild, from the renewed use of old methods (such as tagging or field experiments) to an increasing use of technology-assisted paradigms as well as increasingly large-scale laboratory methods. Overall, the present Feature demonstrates a current drive to introduce holistic approaches for making sense of the social world. Such approaches also require the use of combined integrative and statistical methods. Nevertheless, beyond introducing such methods by leading researchers in the field, we also believe this Special Feature is important in raising the ethical issues that can surround the use of these innovative methods in the field, and as such, will need to be taken into account in a human world that is increasingly aware of its impact on its surrounding wildlife.</p><p>Implementing field experiments has a long history in all sorts of taxa, and they have been used to study social behaviour in wild animals for several decades in some cases (Seyfarth et al., <span>1980</span>). Yet, recent years have allowed the development of increasingly automated methods which minimize interaction between researchers and their study species. For example, in this Special Feature, Wild et al. (<span>2022</span>) show a fully automated two-option foraging device, which can adapt itself to the subject, in this case great tits <i>Parus major</i>. They also stress that a fundamental issue in current research is its cost, and therefore advocate for and demonstrate how to use freely available software to implement such research. The use of remote cameras is also chosen by several other researchers in this Special Feature to limit their impact on animals. Mannion et al. (<span>2022</span>), for example, implement field experiments to study cultural propensities in wild chimpanzees <i>Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii</i>. Nevertheless, they also discuss how much behaviour is lost in the process of solely relying on remote cameras and advocate for a multidimensional approach using both ecological and physiological markers to complement the video data. Remote sensing is also one of several techniques that Sarabian and collaborators (2023) advocate for, to allow the study of disgust amongst an astonishingly large number of species. They combine methods from learning theory with new findings in machine learning to showcase how they can push the study of this highly adaptive emotion in animals. Finally, King and Jensen (<span>2022</span>), tasked with the far from easy challenge of conducting playback experiments with marine mammals, show that promising advances have been made by combining them with remote sensing, particularly the use of drones (see next section) to follow their focal animals. They also discuss the use of non-invasive tagging, which we address in the next paragraph, along with reporting articles from other authors in our Special Feature.</p><p>Tagging animals has an equally long, if not longer history than field experiments for studying social behaviour in wild animals (McIntyre, <span>2014</span>). Here also, while the technology itself is old, the miniaturization of tags, and what they can carry with them, has allowed gathering much more data on the animals besides their identification. Demartsev, Gersick, et al. (<span>2022</span>) show that the future may lie in multi-sensor tracking that can simultaneously record both movements and communication inside a social group. This unprecedented combination may allow understanding much of decision-making in animal groups, given the wealth of data accumulated. But just how much data should we meaningfully consider? He et al. (<span>2022</span>) provide detailed recommendations for implementing GPS-studies, highlighting the major issues regarding sampling, such as the number of animals to consider, how much time tracking has to last, and its frequency. They illustrate those recommendations using their work in vulturine guineafowl <i>Acryllium vulturinum</i>.</p><p>While tags and their development have been privileged for decades, new technology also offers much welcome new avenues of research. Echoing King and Jensen (<span>2022</span>), Schad and Fischer (<span>2022</span>) show how drones can be used to study a range of issues in individual and collective behaviour, particularly when paired with computer algorithms and automated detection software. Importantly, they also discuss the impact of drones in terms of animal disturbance, which we will come back to in our final paragraph. Introducing technology to animals can also be done by presenting touchscreens to animals, as demonstrated by Harrison et al. (<span>2023</span>). Crucially, the use of touchscreens is a tool of choice in captive studies, allowing one to implement similar paradigms as in captivity but with the beneficial ecological validity of wild subjects.</p><p>The introduction of ever-increasing technology may however frighten some field researchers for whom reliance on human-built apparatuses may drive animals out of their natural behaviour. This would threaten the very use of wild subjects as ‘ecologically valid’ by producing artefactual behaviour, rather than the natural repertoire of the species. However, technology can be used differently. Firstly, it can be used to analyse with ever increasing precision the products of field research. Schneider et al. (<span>2023</span>) highlight how environmental DNA can be meaningfully used to test for the presence of intergroup variation in diet in neighbouring vervet monkey groups <i>Chlorocebus pygerythrus</i>. While their results do not allow them to make firm conclusions regarding this question in their study groups, they provide the tools to do so across species. Crucially, these tools may also allow investigation of how much of the diet of these wild animals is impacted by humans themselves: in a nutshell, the future might tell us how much our wild subjects have remained wild in their foods, despite being confronted with increasingly encroaching humans (Gruber et al., <span>2019</span>; McLennan &amp; Hockings, <span>2014</span>). Gräßle et al. (<span>2023</span>) take a radically different approach and also tackle the ‘ecological validity’ of their wild subjects, but this time, by looking at their brains. Much of our knowledge regarding cognitive processes in animals comes from captive subjects, who have been argued by some to be impoverished versions of their wild counterparts (Boesch, <span>2007</span>). The difficult but worthwhile task of extracting, preserving and studying the brains of wild animals will certainly provide answers regarding the differences between captive and wild animals, with consequences on their social lives.</p><p>Another way to make use of new technology to study social behaviour in animals without resorting to using that technology in their natural habitat is the extended use of video databases that can nowadays be computerized and studied with powerful machine learning algorithms. Both Wiltshire et al. (<span>2023</span>) and Schofield et al. (<span>2023</span>) illustrate how this can be done in wild chimpanzees. Schofield and colleagues look at the use of deep learning face recognition models to generate association networks between wild chimpanzees <i>P. t. verus</i> of the same community, Bossou, in New Guinea, over the course of 17 years. The use of such videos, often recorded within the settings of field experiments (Biro et al., <span>2003</span>) is invaluable to track variations in the social habits of the same long-lived individuals. Wiltshire et al. (<span>2023</span>) also use machine learning approaches to study large corpuses of ape datasets but their goal is different, instead aiming to use machine learning to track movement. Movement tracking has recently become particularly of interest in captive studies, with the development of software such as DeepLabCut (Mathis et al., <span>2018</span>). The application of such software on wild data is highly relevant to both reduce the time taken to extract data but also to improve reliability by limiting human error.</p><p>Finally, one cannot analyse the large corpuses of data acquired without statistical methods that are themselves constantly evolving. Complex statistical models have allowed researchers to shed new light on social networks over the last decade (Allen et al., <span>2013</span>; Hobaiter et al., <span>2014</span>; Sosa et al., <span>2021</span>), this being only one example of how statistical models can aid in the analysis of increasingly complex datasets. Both Barrett (<span>2022</span>) and Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson, et al. (<span>2022</span>) illustrate this in their articles. Barrett first tackles the question of having more than two options. Indeed, many famous social learning experiments, both in the wild and captivity, rely on two-choice tasks. But what happens when more than two choices are present? Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson, et al. (<span>2022</span>) tackle a different issue, which is combining various aspects of social life and the resulting data in statistical models. In particular, they investigate the connection between singing patterns in male rock hyraxes <i>Procavia capensis</i> and their reproductive success.</p><p>An important consideration for the future of the study of sociality in the wild is one of ethical practices. This is not to say that new technologies should be the sole drive for researchers to adopt ethical measures in the field. As exemplified by Gruber (<span>2022</span>) in this Feature, as well as Soulsbury et al. (<span>2020</span>) in a recent primer article, ethical issues arise as soon as we deal with wild animals. In particular, Gruber outlines the multiple ways that, similar to biomedical research, field research can be seen as invasive; he introduces the concepts of <i>body</i> invasiveness and <i>none-body</i> invasiveness to tackle these issues. Researchers already being in animals' natural habitat can constitute a stress, advantaging certain individuals over others. Similarly, wearing a tag can represent a burden that will affect an individual's fitness (Soulsbury et al., <span>2020</span>). New technologies, in line with other older direct manipulations of the environment, are likely to elicit stress, fear, or be potential carriers of human diseases (Gruber, <span>2022</span>). But this is not new and should not be a reason to forbid any reliance on such paradigms to study wild animals. Instead, ethical considerations should push researchers to develop their research protocols in view of limiting their impact on wild animals, while still extracting as much information as they can at one time, to avoid the need to re-expose animals indefinitely. Crucially, the methods presented in this Special Feature will all facilitate this, be it by automatizing feeders (Harrison et al., <span>2023</span>; Wild et al., <span>2022</span>), or by making extensive use of remote sensing (He et al., <span>2022</span>; King &amp; Jensen, <span>2022</span>; Mannion et al., <span>2022</span>; Sarabian et al., <span>2023</span>) thus showing that the study of sociality in the wild is not incompatible with the use of contemporary methods. In fact, such methods should be tested to explore the limits of their use on different species and produce reasonable do-or-do not guidelines that can guide the design and implementation of future research.</p><p>As our editorial has shown, and echoing a recent Feature on social networks (Sosa et al., <span>2021</span>), researchers have now developed methods that give them access to an unprecedented amount of data. They are also developing the tools to analyse them in concert. Contemporary methods used in the field echo the ones developed in captivity, fostering dialogue between domains that have been historically separated. Wild animals, becoming more and more subject to anthropogenetic pressures, also change their behaviour. Whether their social behaviour will also become more like captive individuals will clearly need to be investigated in the future, to assess how our own behaviour as a species modifies others' social structures. If the answer is yes, does this mean we can only helplessly witness such changes without acting? While observing animals has been a practice of all human societies through the ages, we now have the capacity to evaluate how much our own behaviour impacts others. This will allow us to reach outside the scientific community and to press for public actors to implement policies based on a wealth of newly accumulated data (Brakes et al., <span>2021</span>). Ultimately, we can only gather such data in the most complete way, to understand and characterize animal societies, and to advocate for measures to be taken in place to conserve both species and habitats. The use of new technologies, for example producing high-quality footage using cameras mounted on drones, can broaden our audience, allowing publicly funded research to be directly observed, sometimes in real time, by the very people who pay for it. In doing so, new technologies can also shorten the distance between researchers and the lay audience, constituting a powerful tool for research and conservation. This Special Feature highlights a few of the tools that can be used so that this can be achieved.</p><p>Thibaud Gruber wrote the initial draft and both Thibaud Gruber &amp; Erica van de Waal reviewed and contributed to the final version.</p><p>The authors declare no conflict of interest.</p><p>There is no data available for this Editorial.</p>","PeriodicalId":208,"journal":{"name":"Methods in Ecology and Evolution","volume":"14 8","pages":"1838-1841"},"PeriodicalIF":6.3000,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/2041-210X.14178","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Striking pay dirt: Contemporary methods for studying animal sociality in the wild\",\"authors\":\"Thibaud Gruber,&nbsp;Erica van de Waal\",\"doi\":\"10.1111/2041-210X.14178\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>From the early Lascaux painters to British naturalists and to modern scientists worldwide, throughout our history, our species has always watched other animals in their natural environment. In doing so, we were able to get a glimpse of the social life of animals from a wide variety of taxa, and to attempt to make sense of it, for all kinds of purposes, be it hunting or scientific knowledge. Often, those various purposes lead to the same outcomes: taking notice of their patterns and habits or recording their communicative displays and making use of them. Observing animals is not an easy task, and making sense of their sociality even less so. While observing wild animals has remained the major channel through which we can make sense of their social lives, humans are additionally aided by an ever-increasing tool set to do so, fuelled by our ever-improving technology as well as our reliance upon it (Henrich, <span>2017</span>). Such technological advances can be seen both through the methods we use when collecting data in the field and the ones we use to analyse the product of our research. The latter can be as diverse as vocal or urine samples, records of distances or interactions between individuals, or choices in a field experiment, and the field is aided greatly by a constant effort in developing new technologies to analyse them.</p><p>Our Joint Special Feature in <i>Methods in Ecology and Evolution</i> and the <i>Journal of Animal Ecology</i> aims to showcase contemporary methods for studying sociality in the wild, from the renewed use of old methods (such as tagging or field experiments) to an increasing use of technology-assisted paradigms as well as increasingly large-scale laboratory methods. Overall, the present Feature demonstrates a current drive to introduce holistic approaches for making sense of the social world. Such approaches also require the use of combined integrative and statistical methods. Nevertheless, beyond introducing such methods by leading researchers in the field, we also believe this Special Feature is important in raising the ethical issues that can surround the use of these innovative methods in the field, and as such, will need to be taken into account in a human world that is increasingly aware of its impact on its surrounding wildlife.</p><p>Implementing field experiments has a long history in all sorts of taxa, and they have been used to study social behaviour in wild animals for several decades in some cases (Seyfarth et al., <span>1980</span>). Yet, recent years have allowed the development of increasingly automated methods which minimize interaction between researchers and their study species. For example, in this Special Feature, Wild et al. (<span>2022</span>) show a fully automated two-option foraging device, which can adapt itself to the subject, in this case great tits <i>Parus major</i>. They also stress that a fundamental issue in current research is its cost, and therefore advocate for and demonstrate how to use freely available software to implement such research. The use of remote cameras is also chosen by several other researchers in this Special Feature to limit their impact on animals. Mannion et al. (<span>2022</span>), for example, implement field experiments to study cultural propensities in wild chimpanzees <i>Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii</i>. Nevertheless, they also discuss how much behaviour is lost in the process of solely relying on remote cameras and advocate for a multidimensional approach using both ecological and physiological markers to complement the video data. Remote sensing is also one of several techniques that Sarabian and collaborators (2023) advocate for, to allow the study of disgust amongst an astonishingly large number of species. They combine methods from learning theory with new findings in machine learning to showcase how they can push the study of this highly adaptive emotion in animals. Finally, King and Jensen (<span>2022</span>), tasked with the far from easy challenge of conducting playback experiments with marine mammals, show that promising advances have been made by combining them with remote sensing, particularly the use of drones (see next section) to follow their focal animals. They also discuss the use of non-invasive tagging, which we address in the next paragraph, along with reporting articles from other authors in our Special Feature.</p><p>Tagging animals has an equally long, if not longer history than field experiments for studying social behaviour in wild animals (McIntyre, <span>2014</span>). Here also, while the technology itself is old, the miniaturization of tags, and what they can carry with them, has allowed gathering much more data on the animals besides their identification. Demartsev, Gersick, et al. (<span>2022</span>) show that the future may lie in multi-sensor tracking that can simultaneously record both movements and communication inside a social group. This unprecedented combination may allow understanding much of decision-making in animal groups, given the wealth of data accumulated. But just how much data should we meaningfully consider? He et al. (<span>2022</span>) provide detailed recommendations for implementing GPS-studies, highlighting the major issues regarding sampling, such as the number of animals to consider, how much time tracking has to last, and its frequency. They illustrate those recommendations using their work in vulturine guineafowl <i>Acryllium vulturinum</i>.</p><p>While tags and their development have been privileged for decades, new technology also offers much welcome new avenues of research. Echoing King and Jensen (<span>2022</span>), Schad and Fischer (<span>2022</span>) show how drones can be used to study a range of issues in individual and collective behaviour, particularly when paired with computer algorithms and automated detection software. Importantly, they also discuss the impact of drones in terms of animal disturbance, which we will come back to in our final paragraph. Introducing technology to animals can also be done by presenting touchscreens to animals, as demonstrated by Harrison et al. (<span>2023</span>). Crucially, the use of touchscreens is a tool of choice in captive studies, allowing one to implement similar paradigms as in captivity but with the beneficial ecological validity of wild subjects.</p><p>The introduction of ever-increasing technology may however frighten some field researchers for whom reliance on human-built apparatuses may drive animals out of their natural behaviour. This would threaten the very use of wild subjects as ‘ecologically valid’ by producing artefactual behaviour, rather than the natural repertoire of the species. However, technology can be used differently. Firstly, it can be used to analyse with ever increasing precision the products of field research. Schneider et al. (<span>2023</span>) highlight how environmental DNA can be meaningfully used to test for the presence of intergroup variation in diet in neighbouring vervet monkey groups <i>Chlorocebus pygerythrus</i>. While their results do not allow them to make firm conclusions regarding this question in their study groups, they provide the tools to do so across species. Crucially, these tools may also allow investigation of how much of the diet of these wild animals is impacted by humans themselves: in a nutshell, the future might tell us how much our wild subjects have remained wild in their foods, despite being confronted with increasingly encroaching humans (Gruber et al., <span>2019</span>; McLennan &amp; Hockings, <span>2014</span>). Gräßle et al. (<span>2023</span>) take a radically different approach and also tackle the ‘ecological validity’ of their wild subjects, but this time, by looking at their brains. Much of our knowledge regarding cognitive processes in animals comes from captive subjects, who have been argued by some to be impoverished versions of their wild counterparts (Boesch, <span>2007</span>). The difficult but worthwhile task of extracting, preserving and studying the brains of wild animals will certainly provide answers regarding the differences between captive and wild animals, with consequences on their social lives.</p><p>Another way to make use of new technology to study social behaviour in animals without resorting to using that technology in their natural habitat is the extended use of video databases that can nowadays be computerized and studied with powerful machine learning algorithms. Both Wiltshire et al. (<span>2023</span>) and Schofield et al. (<span>2023</span>) illustrate how this can be done in wild chimpanzees. Schofield and colleagues look at the use of deep learning face recognition models to generate association networks between wild chimpanzees <i>P. t. verus</i> of the same community, Bossou, in New Guinea, over the course of 17 years. The use of such videos, often recorded within the settings of field experiments (Biro et al., <span>2003</span>) is invaluable to track variations in the social habits of the same long-lived individuals. Wiltshire et al. (<span>2023</span>) also use machine learning approaches to study large corpuses of ape datasets but their goal is different, instead aiming to use machine learning to track movement. Movement tracking has recently become particularly of interest in captive studies, with the development of software such as DeepLabCut (Mathis et al., <span>2018</span>). The application of such software on wild data is highly relevant to both reduce the time taken to extract data but also to improve reliability by limiting human error.</p><p>Finally, one cannot analyse the large corpuses of data acquired without statistical methods that are themselves constantly evolving. Complex statistical models have allowed researchers to shed new light on social networks over the last decade (Allen et al., <span>2013</span>; Hobaiter et al., <span>2014</span>; Sosa et al., <span>2021</span>), this being only one example of how statistical models can aid in the analysis of increasingly complex datasets. Both Barrett (<span>2022</span>) and Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson, et al. (<span>2022</span>) illustrate this in their articles. Barrett first tackles the question of having more than two options. Indeed, many famous social learning experiments, both in the wild and captivity, rely on two-choice tasks. But what happens when more than two choices are present? Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson, et al. (<span>2022</span>) tackle a different issue, which is combining various aspects of social life and the resulting data in statistical models. In particular, they investigate the connection between singing patterns in male rock hyraxes <i>Procavia capensis</i> and their reproductive success.</p><p>An important consideration for the future of the study of sociality in the wild is one of ethical practices. This is not to say that new technologies should be the sole drive for researchers to adopt ethical measures in the field. As exemplified by Gruber (<span>2022</span>) in this Feature, as well as Soulsbury et al. (<span>2020</span>) in a recent primer article, ethical issues arise as soon as we deal with wild animals. In particular, Gruber outlines the multiple ways that, similar to biomedical research, field research can be seen as invasive; he introduces the concepts of <i>body</i> invasiveness and <i>none-body</i> invasiveness to tackle these issues. Researchers already being in animals' natural habitat can constitute a stress, advantaging certain individuals over others. Similarly, wearing a tag can represent a burden that will affect an individual's fitness (Soulsbury et al., <span>2020</span>). New technologies, in line with other older direct manipulations of the environment, are likely to elicit stress, fear, or be potential carriers of human diseases (Gruber, <span>2022</span>). But this is not new and should not be a reason to forbid any reliance on such paradigms to study wild animals. Instead, ethical considerations should push researchers to develop their research protocols in view of limiting their impact on wild animals, while still extracting as much information as they can at one time, to avoid the need to re-expose animals indefinitely. Crucially, the methods presented in this Special Feature will all facilitate this, be it by automatizing feeders (Harrison et al., <span>2023</span>; Wild et al., <span>2022</span>), or by making extensive use of remote sensing (He et al., <span>2022</span>; King &amp; Jensen, <span>2022</span>; Mannion et al., <span>2022</span>; Sarabian et al., <span>2023</span>) thus showing that the study of sociality in the wild is not incompatible with the use of contemporary methods. In fact, such methods should be tested to explore the limits of their use on different species and produce reasonable do-or-do not guidelines that can guide the design and implementation of future research.</p><p>As our editorial has shown, and echoing a recent Feature on social networks (Sosa et al., <span>2021</span>), researchers have now developed methods that give them access to an unprecedented amount of data. They are also developing the tools to analyse them in concert. Contemporary methods used in the field echo the ones developed in captivity, fostering dialogue between domains that have been historically separated. Wild animals, becoming more and more subject to anthropogenetic pressures, also change their behaviour. Whether their social behaviour will also become more like captive individuals will clearly need to be investigated in the future, to assess how our own behaviour as a species modifies others' social structures. If the answer is yes, does this mean we can only helplessly witness such changes without acting? While observing animals has been a practice of all human societies through the ages, we now have the capacity to evaluate how much our own behaviour impacts others. This will allow us to reach outside the scientific community and to press for public actors to implement policies based on a wealth of newly accumulated data (Brakes et al., <span>2021</span>). Ultimately, we can only gather such data in the most complete way, to understand and characterize animal societies, and to advocate for measures to be taken in place to conserve both species and habitats. The use of new technologies, for example producing high-quality footage using cameras mounted on drones, can broaden our audience, allowing publicly funded research to be directly observed, sometimes in real time, by the very people who pay for it. In doing so, new technologies can also shorten the distance between researchers and the lay audience, constituting a powerful tool for research and conservation. This Special Feature highlights a few of the tools that can be used so that this can be achieved.</p><p>Thibaud Gruber wrote the initial draft and both Thibaud Gruber &amp; Erica van de Waal reviewed and contributed to the final version.</p><p>The authors declare no conflict of interest.</p><p>There is no data available for this Editorial.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":208,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Methods in Ecology and Evolution\",\"volume\":\"14 8\",\"pages\":\"1838-1841\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":6.3000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-08-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/2041-210X.14178\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Methods in Ecology and Evolution\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"93\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/2041-210X.14178\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"环境科学与生态学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"ECOLOGY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Methods in Ecology and Evolution","FirstCategoryId":"93","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/2041-210X.14178","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"ECOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

从早期的拉斯科画家到英国博物学家,再到世界各地的现代科学家,纵观人类的历史,人类一直在观察自然环境中的其他动物。这样一来,我们就能从各种各样的分类群中瞥见动物的社会生活,并试图弄清楚它的意义,无论是出于狩猎还是科学知识的目的。通常,这些不同的目的会导致相同的结果:注意他们的模式和习惯,或者记录他们的交流表现并利用它们。观察动物不是一件容易的事,理解它们的社会性就更不容易了。虽然观察野生动物仍然是我们了解它们社会生活的主要渠道,但由于我们不断改进的技术以及我们对它的依赖,人类也得到了不断增加的工具集的帮助(Henrich, 2017)。这种技术进步可以通过我们在现场收集数据时使用的方法和我们用来分析研究成果的方法来看出。后者可以是各种各样的声音或尿液样本,个人之间的距离或相互作用的记录,或现场实验中的选择,并且该领域在不断努力开发新技术来分析它们方面得到了很大的帮助。我们在《生态学和进化方法》和《动物生态学杂志》上的联合专题旨在展示研究野外社会性的当代方法,从旧方法的重新使用(如标记或实地实验)到越来越多地使用技术辅助范例以及越来越大规模的实验室方法。总的来说,本专题展示了当前引入整体方法来理解社会世界的动力。这种方法还需要综合使用综合和统计方法。然而,除了由该领域的领先研究人员介绍这些方法之外,我们还认为,这一专题在提出围绕在该领域使用这些创新方法的伦理问题方面也很重要,因此,在人类世界中,人们越来越意识到其对周围野生动物的影响,因此需要考虑到这一点。在各种分类群中实施实地实验已有很长的历史,在某些情况下,它们已被用于研究野生动物的社会行为几十年(Seyfarth et al., 1980)。然而,近年来已经允许越来越自动化的方法的发展,减少研究人员和他们的研究物种之间的相互作用。例如,在本专题中,Wild等人(2022)展示了一种全自动双选项觅食装置,该装置可以适应主体,在这种情况下,是大山雀。他们还强调,当前研究的一个基本问题是其成本,因此提倡并示范如何使用可免费获得的软件来实施此类研究。在本专题中,其他几位研究人员也选择使用远程摄像机来限制它们对动物的影响。例如,Mannion et al.(2022)通过实地实验研究了野生黑猩猩Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii的文化倾向。尽管如此,他们也讨论了在仅仅依赖远程摄像机的过程中有多少行为丢失,并提倡使用生态和生理标记来补充视频数据的多维方法。遥感也是Sarabian和合作者(2023)提倡的几种技术之一,可以在数量惊人的物种中进行厌恶研究。他们将学习理论的方法与机器学习的新发现结合起来,展示了他们如何推动对动物这种高度适应性情绪的研究。最后,King和Jensen(2022)承担了对海洋哺乳动物进行回放实验的艰巨挑战,他们表明,通过将它们与遥感相结合,特别是使用无人机(见下一节)跟踪他们的焦点动物,已经取得了有希望的进展。他们还讨论了非侵入性标签的使用,我们将在下一段中讨论,并在我们的专题中介绍其他作者的报告文章。标记动物的历史与研究野生动物社会行为的实地实验一样长,如果不是更长的话(McIntyre, 2014)。在这里,虽然技术本身是老旧的,但标签的小型化,以及它们可以携带的东西,已经允许收集更多关于动物的数据,除了它们的身份。Demartsev, Gersick等人(2022)表明,未来可能是多传感器跟踪,可以同时记录社会群体内的运动和通信。考虑到积累的大量数据,这种前所未有的结合可能使我们对动物群体的决策有更多的了解。 但我们究竟应该有意义地考虑多少数据呢?他等人(2022)为实施gps研究提供了详细的建议,强调了有关采样的主要问题,例如要考虑的动物数量、跟踪必须持续多长时间以及频率。他们用秃鹫珍珠鸡丙烯来说明这些建议。虽然标签及其发展已经有几十年的特权,但新技术也提供了非常受欢迎的新的研究途径。与King和Jensen(2022)相呼应,Schad和Fischer(2022)展示了如何使用无人机来研究个人和集体行为中的一系列问题,特别是在与计算机算法和自动检测软件配对时。重要的是,他们还讨论了无人机在动物干扰方面的影响,我们将在最后一段中回到这一点。向动物介绍技术也可以通过向动物展示触摸屏来完成,正如Harrison等人(2023)所展示的那样。至关重要的是,在圈养研究中,触摸屏的使用是一种选择工具,允许人们实施与圈养研究相似的范式,但同时具有野生研究对象的有益生态有效性。然而,不断发展的技术的引入可能会使一些野外研究人员感到害怕,对他们来说,对人造设备的依赖可能会使动物偏离它们的自然行为。这将威胁到野生主体的“生态有效性”,因为它会产生人工行为,而不是物种的自然技能。然而,技术可以有不同的用途。首先,它可以用来分析越来越精确的实地研究成果。Schneider等人(2023)强调了环境DNA如何能够有效地用于测试邻近的长尾猴(Chlorocebus pygerythrus)群体饮食中存在的组间差异。虽然他们的研究结果不能让他们在研究小组中就这个问题得出明确的结论,但他们提供了跨物种研究的工具。至关重要的是,这些工具还可以调查这些野生动物的饮食有多少受到人类自身的影响:简而言之,未来可能会告诉我们,尽管面临着越来越多的人类入侵,但我们的野生动物在食物中保持了多少野性(Gruber et al., 2019;McLennan,。霍金,2014)。Gräßle等人(2023)采取了一种完全不同的方法,也解决了他们的野生对象的“生态有效性”,但这一次,通过观察他们的大脑。我们关于动物认知过程的大部分知识来自圈养动物,一些人认为它们是野生动物的贫困版本(Boesch, 2007)。提取、保存和研究野生动物的大脑这项困难但有价值的任务,肯定会为圈养动物和野生动物之间的差异提供答案,并对它们的社会生活产生影响。另一种利用新技术来研究动物的社会行为而不诉诸于在自然栖息地使用该技术的方法是扩展使用视频数据库,现在可以通过强大的机器学习算法进行计算机化和研究。Wiltshire等人(2023)和Schofield等人(2023)都说明了如何在野生黑猩猩中做到这一点。斯科菲尔德和他的同事们在17年的时间里,研究了深度学习人脸识别模型在新几内亚博苏同一社区的野生黑猩猩之间产生联系网络的方法。这类视频通常是在野外实验环境中录制的(Biro等人,2003年),对于追踪同一长寿个体的社会习惯变化具有不可估量的价值。Wiltshire等人(2023)也使用机器学习方法来研究大型猿数据集,但他们的目标不同,而是旨在使用机器学习来跟踪运动。随着DeepLabCut等软件的开发,运动跟踪最近在圈捕研究中变得特别有趣(Mathis et al., 2018)。这种软件在原始数据上的应用,不仅可以减少提取数据所需的时间,还可以通过限制人为错误来提高可靠性。最后,如果没有统计方法,就无法分析获得的大量数据,而统计方法本身也在不断发展。在过去十年中,复杂的统计模型让研究人员对社交网络有了新的认识(Allen et al., 2013;Hobaiter et al., 2014;Sosa et al., 2021),这只是统计模型如何帮助分析日益复杂的数据集的一个例子。Barrett(2022)和Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson等人(2022)在他们的文章中都说明了这一点。巴雷特首先解决了有两个以上选择的问题。 事实上,许多著名的社会学习实验,无论是在野外还是在圈养环境中,都依赖于两项选择任务。但是当有两个以上的选择时,会发生什么呢?Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson等人(2022)解决了一个不同的问题,即将社会生活的各个方面和统计模型中的结果数据结合起来。他们特别研究了雄性岩狸的鸣叫模式与繁殖成功率之间的关系。对未来野外社会性研究的一个重要考虑是伦理实践之一。这并不是说新技术应该是研究人员在该领域采取伦理措施的唯一动力。正如格鲁伯(2022)在本专题中的例子,以及索尔斯伯里等人(2020)在最近的一篇入门文章中所阐述的那样,只要我们处理野生动物,伦理问题就会出现。格鲁伯特别概述了与生物医学研究类似的多种方式,实地研究可以被视为侵入性的;他引入了身体侵入性和无身体侵入性的概念来解决这些问题。研究人员已经进入动物的自然栖息地,这可能构成一种压力,使某些个体比其他个体更有利。同样,佩戴标签可能是一种负担,会影响个人的健康(Soulsbury et al., 2020)。新技术,与其他旧的对环境的直接操纵一致,可能会引起压力、恐惧,或成为人类疾病的潜在载体(Gruber, 2022)。但这并不新鲜,也不应该成为禁止依赖这种范式来研究野生动物的理由。相反,伦理方面的考虑应该促使研究人员制定他们的研究方案,以限制他们对野生动物的影响,同时仍然可以一次提取尽可能多的信息,以避免无限期地重新暴露动物。至关重要的是,本专题中提出的方法都将促进这一点,无论是通过自动化馈线(Harrison et al., 2023;Wild et al., 2022),或者通过广泛利用遥感(He et al., 2022;王,詹森,2022;Mannion et al., 2022;Sarabian et al., 2023),因此表明对野生社会性的研究与当代方法的使用并不矛盾。事实上,应该对这些方法进行测试,以探索它们在不同物种上的使用限制,并产生合理的“做或不做”指导方针,从而指导未来研究的设计和实施。正如我们的社论所显示的,以及最近在社交网络上的一篇文章(Sosa et al., 2021),研究人员现在已经开发出了使他们能够访问前所未有的大量数据的方法。他们也在开发工具来共同分析它们。现场使用的当代方法与圈养中开发的方法相呼应,促进了历史上分离的领域之间的对话。野生动物越来越受到人类活动的压力,也改变了它们的行为。它们的社会行为是否也会变得更像圈养的个体,显然需要在未来进行调查,以评估我们作为一个物种的行为如何改变其他物种的社会结构。如果答案是肯定的,这是否意味着我们只能无助地目睹这些变化而不采取行动?虽然观察动物一直是所有人类社会的惯例,但我们现在有能力评估自己的行为对他人的影响程度。这将使我们能够接触到科学界以外的领域,并敦促公共行为体根据大量新积累的数据实施政策(Brakes et al., 2021)。最终,我们只能以最完整的方式收集这些数据,以了解和描述动物社会,并倡导采取措施保护物种和栖息地。新技术的使用,例如使用安装在无人机上的摄像机制作高质量的镜头,可以扩大我们的受众,使公共资助的研究可以直接被观察到,有时是实时的,由支付研究费用的人来观察。在这样做的过程中,新技术还可以缩短研究人员和非专业观众之间的距离,构成研究和保护的有力工具。这个特殊功能突出了一些可以用来实现这一目标的工具。Thibaud Gruber写了初稿,Thibaud Gruber &Erica van de Waal审阅并贡献了最终版本。作者声明无利益冲突。这篇社论没有可用的数据。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Striking pay dirt: Contemporary methods for studying animal sociality in the wild

From the early Lascaux painters to British naturalists and to modern scientists worldwide, throughout our history, our species has always watched other animals in their natural environment. In doing so, we were able to get a glimpse of the social life of animals from a wide variety of taxa, and to attempt to make sense of it, for all kinds of purposes, be it hunting or scientific knowledge. Often, those various purposes lead to the same outcomes: taking notice of their patterns and habits or recording their communicative displays and making use of them. Observing animals is not an easy task, and making sense of their sociality even less so. While observing wild animals has remained the major channel through which we can make sense of their social lives, humans are additionally aided by an ever-increasing tool set to do so, fuelled by our ever-improving technology as well as our reliance upon it (Henrich, 2017). Such technological advances can be seen both through the methods we use when collecting data in the field and the ones we use to analyse the product of our research. The latter can be as diverse as vocal or urine samples, records of distances or interactions between individuals, or choices in a field experiment, and the field is aided greatly by a constant effort in developing new technologies to analyse them.

Our Joint Special Feature in Methods in Ecology and Evolution and the Journal of Animal Ecology aims to showcase contemporary methods for studying sociality in the wild, from the renewed use of old methods (such as tagging or field experiments) to an increasing use of technology-assisted paradigms as well as increasingly large-scale laboratory methods. Overall, the present Feature demonstrates a current drive to introduce holistic approaches for making sense of the social world. Such approaches also require the use of combined integrative and statistical methods. Nevertheless, beyond introducing such methods by leading researchers in the field, we also believe this Special Feature is important in raising the ethical issues that can surround the use of these innovative methods in the field, and as such, will need to be taken into account in a human world that is increasingly aware of its impact on its surrounding wildlife.

Implementing field experiments has a long history in all sorts of taxa, and they have been used to study social behaviour in wild animals for several decades in some cases (Seyfarth et al., 1980). Yet, recent years have allowed the development of increasingly automated methods which minimize interaction between researchers and their study species. For example, in this Special Feature, Wild et al. (2022) show a fully automated two-option foraging device, which can adapt itself to the subject, in this case great tits Parus major. They also stress that a fundamental issue in current research is its cost, and therefore advocate for and demonstrate how to use freely available software to implement such research. The use of remote cameras is also chosen by several other researchers in this Special Feature to limit their impact on animals. Mannion et al. (2022), for example, implement field experiments to study cultural propensities in wild chimpanzees Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Nevertheless, they also discuss how much behaviour is lost in the process of solely relying on remote cameras and advocate for a multidimensional approach using both ecological and physiological markers to complement the video data. Remote sensing is also one of several techniques that Sarabian and collaborators (2023) advocate for, to allow the study of disgust amongst an astonishingly large number of species. They combine methods from learning theory with new findings in machine learning to showcase how they can push the study of this highly adaptive emotion in animals. Finally, King and Jensen (2022), tasked with the far from easy challenge of conducting playback experiments with marine mammals, show that promising advances have been made by combining them with remote sensing, particularly the use of drones (see next section) to follow their focal animals. They also discuss the use of non-invasive tagging, which we address in the next paragraph, along with reporting articles from other authors in our Special Feature.

Tagging animals has an equally long, if not longer history than field experiments for studying social behaviour in wild animals (McIntyre, 2014). Here also, while the technology itself is old, the miniaturization of tags, and what they can carry with them, has allowed gathering much more data on the animals besides their identification. Demartsev, Gersick, et al. (2022) show that the future may lie in multi-sensor tracking that can simultaneously record both movements and communication inside a social group. This unprecedented combination may allow understanding much of decision-making in animal groups, given the wealth of data accumulated. But just how much data should we meaningfully consider? He et al. (2022) provide detailed recommendations for implementing GPS-studies, highlighting the major issues regarding sampling, such as the number of animals to consider, how much time tracking has to last, and its frequency. They illustrate those recommendations using their work in vulturine guineafowl Acryllium vulturinum.

While tags and their development have been privileged for decades, new technology also offers much welcome new avenues of research. Echoing King and Jensen (2022), Schad and Fischer (2022) show how drones can be used to study a range of issues in individual and collective behaviour, particularly when paired with computer algorithms and automated detection software. Importantly, they also discuss the impact of drones in terms of animal disturbance, which we will come back to in our final paragraph. Introducing technology to animals can also be done by presenting touchscreens to animals, as demonstrated by Harrison et al. (2023). Crucially, the use of touchscreens is a tool of choice in captive studies, allowing one to implement similar paradigms as in captivity but with the beneficial ecological validity of wild subjects.

The introduction of ever-increasing technology may however frighten some field researchers for whom reliance on human-built apparatuses may drive animals out of their natural behaviour. This would threaten the very use of wild subjects as ‘ecologically valid’ by producing artefactual behaviour, rather than the natural repertoire of the species. However, technology can be used differently. Firstly, it can be used to analyse with ever increasing precision the products of field research. Schneider et al. (2023) highlight how environmental DNA can be meaningfully used to test for the presence of intergroup variation in diet in neighbouring vervet monkey groups Chlorocebus pygerythrus. While their results do not allow them to make firm conclusions regarding this question in their study groups, they provide the tools to do so across species. Crucially, these tools may also allow investigation of how much of the diet of these wild animals is impacted by humans themselves: in a nutshell, the future might tell us how much our wild subjects have remained wild in their foods, despite being confronted with increasingly encroaching humans (Gruber et al., 2019; McLennan & Hockings, 2014). Gräßle et al. (2023) take a radically different approach and also tackle the ‘ecological validity’ of their wild subjects, but this time, by looking at their brains. Much of our knowledge regarding cognitive processes in animals comes from captive subjects, who have been argued by some to be impoverished versions of their wild counterparts (Boesch, 2007). The difficult but worthwhile task of extracting, preserving and studying the brains of wild animals will certainly provide answers regarding the differences between captive and wild animals, with consequences on their social lives.

Another way to make use of new technology to study social behaviour in animals without resorting to using that technology in their natural habitat is the extended use of video databases that can nowadays be computerized and studied with powerful machine learning algorithms. Both Wiltshire et al. (2023) and Schofield et al. (2023) illustrate how this can be done in wild chimpanzees. Schofield and colleagues look at the use of deep learning face recognition models to generate association networks between wild chimpanzees P. t. verus of the same community, Bossou, in New Guinea, over the course of 17 years. The use of such videos, often recorded within the settings of field experiments (Biro et al., 2003) is invaluable to track variations in the social habits of the same long-lived individuals. Wiltshire et al. (2023) also use machine learning approaches to study large corpuses of ape datasets but their goal is different, instead aiming to use machine learning to track movement. Movement tracking has recently become particularly of interest in captive studies, with the development of software such as DeepLabCut (Mathis et al., 2018). The application of such software on wild data is highly relevant to both reduce the time taken to extract data but also to improve reliability by limiting human error.

Finally, one cannot analyse the large corpuses of data acquired without statistical methods that are themselves constantly evolving. Complex statistical models have allowed researchers to shed new light on social networks over the last decade (Allen et al., 2013; Hobaiter et al., 2014; Sosa et al., 2021), this being only one example of how statistical models can aid in the analysis of increasingly complex datasets. Both Barrett (2022) and Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson, et al. (2022) illustrate this in their articles. Barrett first tackles the question of having more than two options. Indeed, many famous social learning experiments, both in the wild and captivity, rely on two-choice tasks. But what happens when more than two choices are present? Demartsev, Haddas-Sasson, et al. (2022) tackle a different issue, which is combining various aspects of social life and the resulting data in statistical models. In particular, they investigate the connection between singing patterns in male rock hyraxes Procavia capensis and their reproductive success.

An important consideration for the future of the study of sociality in the wild is one of ethical practices. This is not to say that new technologies should be the sole drive for researchers to adopt ethical measures in the field. As exemplified by Gruber (2022) in this Feature, as well as Soulsbury et al. (2020) in a recent primer article, ethical issues arise as soon as we deal with wild animals. In particular, Gruber outlines the multiple ways that, similar to biomedical research, field research can be seen as invasive; he introduces the concepts of body invasiveness and none-body invasiveness to tackle these issues. Researchers already being in animals' natural habitat can constitute a stress, advantaging certain individuals over others. Similarly, wearing a tag can represent a burden that will affect an individual's fitness (Soulsbury et al., 2020). New technologies, in line with other older direct manipulations of the environment, are likely to elicit stress, fear, or be potential carriers of human diseases (Gruber, 2022). But this is not new and should not be a reason to forbid any reliance on such paradigms to study wild animals. Instead, ethical considerations should push researchers to develop their research protocols in view of limiting their impact on wild animals, while still extracting as much information as they can at one time, to avoid the need to re-expose animals indefinitely. Crucially, the methods presented in this Special Feature will all facilitate this, be it by automatizing feeders (Harrison et al., 2023; Wild et al., 2022), or by making extensive use of remote sensing (He et al., 2022; King & Jensen, 2022; Mannion et al., 2022; Sarabian et al., 2023) thus showing that the study of sociality in the wild is not incompatible with the use of contemporary methods. In fact, such methods should be tested to explore the limits of their use on different species and produce reasonable do-or-do not guidelines that can guide the design and implementation of future research.

As our editorial has shown, and echoing a recent Feature on social networks (Sosa et al., 2021), researchers have now developed methods that give them access to an unprecedented amount of data. They are also developing the tools to analyse them in concert. Contemporary methods used in the field echo the ones developed in captivity, fostering dialogue between domains that have been historically separated. Wild animals, becoming more and more subject to anthropogenetic pressures, also change their behaviour. Whether their social behaviour will also become more like captive individuals will clearly need to be investigated in the future, to assess how our own behaviour as a species modifies others' social structures. If the answer is yes, does this mean we can only helplessly witness such changes without acting? While observing animals has been a practice of all human societies through the ages, we now have the capacity to evaluate how much our own behaviour impacts others. This will allow us to reach outside the scientific community and to press for public actors to implement policies based on a wealth of newly accumulated data (Brakes et al., 2021). Ultimately, we can only gather such data in the most complete way, to understand and characterize animal societies, and to advocate for measures to be taken in place to conserve both species and habitats. The use of new technologies, for example producing high-quality footage using cameras mounted on drones, can broaden our audience, allowing publicly funded research to be directly observed, sometimes in real time, by the very people who pay for it. In doing so, new technologies can also shorten the distance between researchers and the lay audience, constituting a powerful tool for research and conservation. This Special Feature highlights a few of the tools that can be used so that this can be achieved.

Thibaud Gruber wrote the initial draft and both Thibaud Gruber & Erica van de Waal reviewed and contributed to the final version.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

There is no data available for this Editorial.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
11.60
自引率
3.00%
发文量
236
审稿时长
4-8 weeks
期刊介绍: A British Ecological Society journal, Methods in Ecology and Evolution (MEE) promotes the development of new methods in ecology and evolution, and facilitates their dissemination and uptake by the research community. MEE brings together papers from previously disparate sub-disciplines to provide a single forum for tracking methodological developments in all areas. MEE publishes methodological papers in any area of ecology and evolution, including: -Phylogenetic analysis -Statistical methods -Conservation & management -Theoretical methods -Practical methods, including lab and field -This list is not exhaustive, and we welcome enquiries about possible submissions. Methods are defined in the widest terms and may be analytical, practical or conceptual. A primary aim of the journal is to maximise the uptake of techniques by the community. We recognise that a major stumbling block in the uptake and application of new methods is the accessibility of methods. For example, users may need computer code, example applications or demonstrations of methods.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信