{"title":"不同强化剂相对有效性的比较:PEM方法。","authors":"Hsen-Hsing Ma","doi":"10.1037/H0100680","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Introduction In educational settings, it is desired that all students know the value of knowledge and skills and are intrinsically motivated to learn, but it is rarely the case. Therefore, teachers in school settings often have to employ extrinsic reinforcers to motivate students to learn. After the frequent use of reinforcement on good school performance, it can be expected that the act of learning will become a conditioned response, and after the teacher applies intermittent reinforcement or the student acquires natural consequence of success in education or carrier, it is hoped that the learning can become intrinsically motivated with less extrinsic reinforcement. Reinforcement can be classified into four kinds: (a) positive reinforcement (giving positive reinforcer), (b) punishment (giving negative reinforcer), (c) punishment (withdrawing positive reinforcer), and (d) negative reinforcement (withdrawing negative reinforcer). The present study concentrated mostly on the comparison of the effectiveness of positive reinforcers including edible foods, tangible objects, activities, and tokens. Less attention is paid to the effectiveness of negative (aversive) reinforcers, but the mean effect sizes of punishment were presented for the purpose of comparison. There have been many studies reporting success in the use of primary reinforcers to modify the behavior of participants (e.g., Forness, Kavale, Blum, and Lloyd, 1997; Kern, Ringdahl, Hilt, and Sterling-Turner, 2001; Osborne, 1969; and Williams, Koegel, and Egel, 1981). Cameron and Pierce (1994) conducted a meta-analysis to address the issue of whether extrinsic reinforcement is harmful to the intrinsic motivation and found that rewards given for task completion or for quality of performance are not detrimental to intrinsic motivation. According to the principle of behavior modification, in order to expect a desirable behavior to happen in the future, three conditions must be fulfilled as follows: a discriminative stimulus must be present; there must be a contingency for reinforcement of the target behavior, and the reinforcer must be able to satisfy the need of the individual. Researchers in behavior analysis have paid more attention to the third condition recently. Neef and Lutz (2001) found that the effect of more preferred reinforcers was higher than that of less preferred reinforcers. The results of Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, and Page's (1985) study confirmed that the success of reinforcement depends on the selection of suitable reinforcement schedules and contingencies. Glynn (1970) found that the effect of self-determined and experimenter-determined token intervention on the learning of history and geography material was superior to that of chance-determined and no-token interventions. It may be alternatively hypothesized that the effect size of an intervention would be larger when the reinforcer can better meet the needs of the participant and serve as a mechanism to increase his or her motivation. The tool used to measure the effectiveness of different reinforcers was the percentage of data points exceeding the median of baseline phase (PEM) approach (Ma, 2006). By far, the most widely used method for measuring the effect size from single-case experimental designs is the percentage of nonoverlapping data (PND) approach proposed by Mastropieri and Scruggs (1985-1986). The strength and weakness of the PEM approach and its superiority over the PND method has been discussed by Ma (2006) and empirically confirmed by Gao and Ma (2006); Chen and Ma (2007); Ma (2009) and Preston and Carter (2009). Therefore, it was decided to apply the PEM approach to compare the relative effectiveness of different reinforcers used in the field of behavior modification. Method Procedures for Locating Studies The single -case experimental studies investigating the effect of reinforcers analyzed in this synthesis were obtained through a computer-assisted search of the relevant databases, including EBSCOhost, ERIC, and ProQuest. …","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"10 1","pages":"398-427"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2010-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"5","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Comparison of the Relative Effectiveness of Different Kinds of Reinforcers: A PEM Approach.\",\"authors\":\"Hsen-Hsing Ma\",\"doi\":\"10.1037/H0100680\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Introduction In educational settings, it is desired that all students know the value of knowledge and skills and are intrinsically motivated to learn, but it is rarely the case. Therefore, teachers in school settings often have to employ extrinsic reinforcers to motivate students to learn. After the frequent use of reinforcement on good school performance, it can be expected that the act of learning will become a conditioned response, and after the teacher applies intermittent reinforcement or the student acquires natural consequence of success in education or carrier, it is hoped that the learning can become intrinsically motivated with less extrinsic reinforcement. Reinforcement can be classified into four kinds: (a) positive reinforcement (giving positive reinforcer), (b) punishment (giving negative reinforcer), (c) punishment (withdrawing positive reinforcer), and (d) negative reinforcement (withdrawing negative reinforcer). The present study concentrated mostly on the comparison of the effectiveness of positive reinforcers including edible foods, tangible objects, activities, and tokens. Less attention is paid to the effectiveness of negative (aversive) reinforcers, but the mean effect sizes of punishment were presented for the purpose of comparison. There have been many studies reporting success in the use of primary reinforcers to modify the behavior of participants (e.g., Forness, Kavale, Blum, and Lloyd, 1997; Kern, Ringdahl, Hilt, and Sterling-Turner, 2001; Osborne, 1969; and Williams, Koegel, and Egel, 1981). Cameron and Pierce (1994) conducted a meta-analysis to address the issue of whether extrinsic reinforcement is harmful to the intrinsic motivation and found that rewards given for task completion or for quality of performance are not detrimental to intrinsic motivation. According to the principle of behavior modification, in order to expect a desirable behavior to happen in the future, three conditions must be fulfilled as follows: a discriminative stimulus must be present; there must be a contingency for reinforcement of the target behavior, and the reinforcer must be able to satisfy the need of the individual. Researchers in behavior analysis have paid more attention to the third condition recently. Neef and Lutz (2001) found that the effect of more preferred reinforcers was higher than that of less preferred reinforcers. The results of Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, and Page's (1985) study confirmed that the success of reinforcement depends on the selection of suitable reinforcement schedules and contingencies. Glynn (1970) found that the effect of self-determined and experimenter-determined token intervention on the learning of history and geography material was superior to that of chance-determined and no-token interventions. It may be alternatively hypothesized that the effect size of an intervention would be larger when the reinforcer can better meet the needs of the participant and serve as a mechanism to increase his or her motivation. The tool used to measure the effectiveness of different reinforcers was the percentage of data points exceeding the median of baseline phase (PEM) approach (Ma, 2006). By far, the most widely used method for measuring the effect size from single-case experimental designs is the percentage of nonoverlapping data (PND) approach proposed by Mastropieri and Scruggs (1985-1986). The strength and weakness of the PEM approach and its superiority over the PND method has been discussed by Ma (2006) and empirically confirmed by Gao and Ma (2006); Chen and Ma (2007); Ma (2009) and Preston and Carter (2009). Therefore, it was decided to apply the PEM approach to compare the relative effectiveness of different reinforcers used in the field of behavior modification. Method Procedures for Locating Studies The single -case experimental studies investigating the effect of reinforcers analyzed in this synthesis were obtained through a computer-assisted search of the relevant databases, including EBSCOhost, ERIC, and ProQuest. …\",\"PeriodicalId\":88717,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"The behavior analyst today\",\"volume\":\"10 1\",\"pages\":\"398-427\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2010-06-22\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"5\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"The behavior analyst today\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100680\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The behavior analyst today","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100680","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
摘要
在教育环境中,人们希望所有的学生都知道知识和技能的价值,并有内在的动力去学习,但这种情况很少发生。因此,教师在学校设置往往不得不采用外部强化激励学生学习。在对良好学习表现进行频繁强化后,可以预期学习行为将成为条件反应,在教师进行间歇性强化或学生获得教育成功或载体的自然结果后,希望学习成为内在动机,减少外在强化。强化可分为四种:(a)正强化(给予正强化),(b)惩罚(给予负强化),(c)惩罚(撤回正强化),(d)负强化(撤回负强化)。本研究主要集中在食用食物、有形物品、活动和标记物等正向强化物的有效性比较上。对负强化(厌恶强化)的有效性关注较少,但为了进行比较,我们给出了惩罚的平均效应量。有许多研究报告成功地使用初级强化物来改变参与者的行为(例如,Forness, Kavale, Blum, and Lloyd, 1997;Kern, Ringdahl, Hilt, and Sterling-Turner, 2001;奥斯本1969;和Williams, Koegel, and Egel, 1981)。Cameron和Pierce(1994)进行了一项元分析,以解决外部强化是否对内在动机有害的问题,并发现对任务完成或绩效质量的奖励对内在动机并不有害。根据行为修正原理,为了期望未来发生理想的行为,必须满足以下三个条件:必须存在判别刺激;目标行为的强化必须有偶然性,强化者必须能够满足个体的需要。最近,行为分析的研究者们更加关注第三种情况。Neef和Lutz(2001)发现偏好强化物较多的效应高于偏好强化物较少的效应。Pace、Ivancic、Edwards、Iwata和Page(1985)的研究结果证实,强化的成功取决于选择合适的强化计划和偶发性。Glynn(1970)发现,自我决定的和实验者决定的象征性干预对历史地理资料学习的效果优于机会决定的和无象征性干预。另一种假设是,当强化物能够更好地满足被试的需求,并作为一种增加被试动机的机制时,干预的效应量就会更大。用于衡量不同强化剂有效性的工具是超过基线阶段(PEM)方法中位数的数据点百分比(Ma, 2006)。迄今为止,最广泛使用的测量单例实验设计效应大小的方法是由马斯特罗皮里和斯克鲁格斯(1985-1986)提出的非重叠数据百分比(PND)方法。PEM方法的优缺点及其相对于PND方法的优越性已经被Ma(2006)讨论过,并得到Gao和Ma(2006)的经验证实;陈、马(2007);Ma(2009)和Preston and Carter(2009)。因此,决定采用PEM方法来比较行为矫正领域中使用的不同强化物的相对有效性。通过计算机辅助检索相关数据库,包括EBSCOhost、ERIC和ProQuest,获得本综合研究中分析的强化剂影响的单例实验研究。...
Comparison of the Relative Effectiveness of Different Kinds of Reinforcers: A PEM Approach.
Introduction In educational settings, it is desired that all students know the value of knowledge and skills and are intrinsically motivated to learn, but it is rarely the case. Therefore, teachers in school settings often have to employ extrinsic reinforcers to motivate students to learn. After the frequent use of reinforcement on good school performance, it can be expected that the act of learning will become a conditioned response, and after the teacher applies intermittent reinforcement or the student acquires natural consequence of success in education or carrier, it is hoped that the learning can become intrinsically motivated with less extrinsic reinforcement. Reinforcement can be classified into four kinds: (a) positive reinforcement (giving positive reinforcer), (b) punishment (giving negative reinforcer), (c) punishment (withdrawing positive reinforcer), and (d) negative reinforcement (withdrawing negative reinforcer). The present study concentrated mostly on the comparison of the effectiveness of positive reinforcers including edible foods, tangible objects, activities, and tokens. Less attention is paid to the effectiveness of negative (aversive) reinforcers, but the mean effect sizes of punishment were presented for the purpose of comparison. There have been many studies reporting success in the use of primary reinforcers to modify the behavior of participants (e.g., Forness, Kavale, Blum, and Lloyd, 1997; Kern, Ringdahl, Hilt, and Sterling-Turner, 2001; Osborne, 1969; and Williams, Koegel, and Egel, 1981). Cameron and Pierce (1994) conducted a meta-analysis to address the issue of whether extrinsic reinforcement is harmful to the intrinsic motivation and found that rewards given for task completion or for quality of performance are not detrimental to intrinsic motivation. According to the principle of behavior modification, in order to expect a desirable behavior to happen in the future, three conditions must be fulfilled as follows: a discriminative stimulus must be present; there must be a contingency for reinforcement of the target behavior, and the reinforcer must be able to satisfy the need of the individual. Researchers in behavior analysis have paid more attention to the third condition recently. Neef and Lutz (2001) found that the effect of more preferred reinforcers was higher than that of less preferred reinforcers. The results of Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, and Page's (1985) study confirmed that the success of reinforcement depends on the selection of suitable reinforcement schedules and contingencies. Glynn (1970) found that the effect of self-determined and experimenter-determined token intervention on the learning of history and geography material was superior to that of chance-determined and no-token interventions. It may be alternatively hypothesized that the effect size of an intervention would be larger when the reinforcer can better meet the needs of the participant and serve as a mechanism to increase his or her motivation. The tool used to measure the effectiveness of different reinforcers was the percentage of data points exceeding the median of baseline phase (PEM) approach (Ma, 2006). By far, the most widely used method for measuring the effect size from single-case experimental designs is the percentage of nonoverlapping data (PND) approach proposed by Mastropieri and Scruggs (1985-1986). The strength and weakness of the PEM approach and its superiority over the PND method has been discussed by Ma (2006) and empirically confirmed by Gao and Ma (2006); Chen and Ma (2007); Ma (2009) and Preston and Carter (2009). Therefore, it was decided to apply the PEM approach to compare the relative effectiveness of different reinforcers used in the field of behavior modification. Method Procedures for Locating Studies The single -case experimental studies investigating the effect of reinforcers analyzed in this synthesis were obtained through a computer-assisted search of the relevant databases, including EBSCOhost, ERIC, and ProQuest. …