灭绝诱导的反应复苏:选择性回顾

Adam H. Doughty, Gabriella Oken
{"title":"灭绝诱导的反应复苏:选择性回顾","authors":"Adam H. Doughty, Gabriella Oken","doi":"10.1037/H0100644","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Resurgence refers to the recovery of previously extinguished responding when a recently reinforced response is extinguished (e.g., Epstein, 1983, 1985). Figure 1 illustrates resurgence using hypothetical data. In the first condition, only Response A is reinforced. Response A is extinguished in the second condition concurrent with the reinforcement of Response B. Response B is extinguished in the third condition, and the subsequent recovery of Response A is resurgence. The degree of recovery of Response A can be compared to a different (control) response that never was reinforced (Response C). Measuring Response C in this third condition serves as a measure of extinction-induced response variability (e.g., Antonitis, 1951). Thus, resurgence provides a more refined characterization of extinction-induced behavior. That is, responses predicted to occur in extinction are those responses that previously were reinforced under comparable stimulus conditions. Although resurgence and related extinction-induced phenomena were investigated in the 1970s (e.g., Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976; Pacitti, & Smith, 1977; Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977), as well as earlier (see Epstein, 1985), Epstein (1983, 1985) usually is viewed as being the most forceful commentator regarding the utility of understanding the determinants and consequences of resurgence (see also Epstein & Skinner, 1980). Epstein argued that resurgence provides behavior analysts with a more descriptive means of understanding findings sometimes attributed to Freud's concept of regression. For Freud, regression involved an individual regressing to previously demonstrated behavior considered acceptable earlier in the individual's development. In addition to linking resurgence and regression, Epstein also summarized much of the experimental findings related to resurgence. The aim of the present paper, therefore, is to describe more recent experimental findings related to resurgence. By showing the relevance of the topic to a variety of topics in and outside behavior analysis (behavioral history, drug relapse, severe problem behavior, communication disorders, and cognition), this paper urges both basic and applied behavior analysts to continue the analysis of resurgence. Behavioral history Appealing to the behavioral history of an organism often provides behavior analysts with a means of explaining complex behavior (e.g., Barrett, 1986; Doughty, Cirino, Mayfield, da Silva, Okouchi, & Lattal, 2005; Lattal, & Neef, 1996; Tatham, & Wanchisen, 1998; Wanchisen, & Tatham, 1991). Broadly speaking, resurgence is a behavioral-history effect in that current behavior only can be understood by appealing to contingencies of reinforcement exposed to the organism previously. A primary issue, therefore, in the study of resurgence is: what are the necessary and sufficient conditions to produce resurgence, in terms of both past experiences and current environmental conditions? Lieving and Lattal (2003) addressed the question posed above across several experiments. In one experiment, key pecking of pigeons was reinforced via grain delivery in the first condition. In the second condition, key pecking was extinguished concurrent with the reinforcement of treadle pressing (also via grain delivery). In the third condition, treadle pressing was extinguished, and the resurgence of key pecking was measured. By repeating these three conditions , and consistently observing key-peck resurgence in the third condition, it was confirmed that resurgence is a general and replicable finding. In two subsequent experiments, the necessity of extinguishing treadle pressing in producing key-peck resurgence was investigated. In one experiment, instead of arranging extinction in the third condition, food delivery was made response independent. Not only was key-peck resurgence absent in this condition, but in a subsequent condition key pecking resurged when extinction replaced the response-independent, food-delivery schedule. …","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"27-33"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2008-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"46","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Extinction-Induced Response Resurgence: A Selective Review\",\"authors\":\"Adam H. Doughty, Gabriella Oken\",\"doi\":\"10.1037/H0100644\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Resurgence refers to the recovery of previously extinguished responding when a recently reinforced response is extinguished (e.g., Epstein, 1983, 1985). Figure 1 illustrates resurgence using hypothetical data. In the first condition, only Response A is reinforced. Response A is extinguished in the second condition concurrent with the reinforcement of Response B. Response B is extinguished in the third condition, and the subsequent recovery of Response A is resurgence. The degree of recovery of Response A can be compared to a different (control) response that never was reinforced (Response C). Measuring Response C in this third condition serves as a measure of extinction-induced response variability (e.g., Antonitis, 1951). Thus, resurgence provides a more refined characterization of extinction-induced behavior. That is, responses predicted to occur in extinction are those responses that previously were reinforced under comparable stimulus conditions. Although resurgence and related extinction-induced phenomena were investigated in the 1970s (e.g., Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976; Pacitti, & Smith, 1977; Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977), as well as earlier (see Epstein, 1985), Epstein (1983, 1985) usually is viewed as being the most forceful commentator regarding the utility of understanding the determinants and consequences of resurgence (see also Epstein & Skinner, 1980). Epstein argued that resurgence provides behavior analysts with a more descriptive means of understanding findings sometimes attributed to Freud's concept of regression. For Freud, regression involved an individual regressing to previously demonstrated behavior considered acceptable earlier in the individual's development. In addition to linking resurgence and regression, Epstein also summarized much of the experimental findings related to resurgence. The aim of the present paper, therefore, is to describe more recent experimental findings related to resurgence. By showing the relevance of the topic to a variety of topics in and outside behavior analysis (behavioral history, drug relapse, severe problem behavior, communication disorders, and cognition), this paper urges both basic and applied behavior analysts to continue the analysis of resurgence. Behavioral history Appealing to the behavioral history of an organism often provides behavior analysts with a means of explaining complex behavior (e.g., Barrett, 1986; Doughty, Cirino, Mayfield, da Silva, Okouchi, & Lattal, 2005; Lattal, & Neef, 1996; Tatham, & Wanchisen, 1998; Wanchisen, & Tatham, 1991). Broadly speaking, resurgence is a behavioral-history effect in that current behavior only can be understood by appealing to contingencies of reinforcement exposed to the organism previously. A primary issue, therefore, in the study of resurgence is: what are the necessary and sufficient conditions to produce resurgence, in terms of both past experiences and current environmental conditions? Lieving and Lattal (2003) addressed the question posed above across several experiments. In one experiment, key pecking of pigeons was reinforced via grain delivery in the first condition. In the second condition, key pecking was extinguished concurrent with the reinforcement of treadle pressing (also via grain delivery). In the third condition, treadle pressing was extinguished, and the resurgence of key pecking was measured. By repeating these three conditions , and consistently observing key-peck resurgence in the third condition, it was confirmed that resurgence is a general and replicable finding. In two subsequent experiments, the necessity of extinguishing treadle pressing in producing key-peck resurgence was investigated. In one experiment, instead of arranging extinction in the third condition, food delivery was made response independent. Not only was key-peck resurgence absent in this condition, but in a subsequent condition key pecking resurged when extinction replaced the response-independent, food-delivery schedule. …\",\"PeriodicalId\":88717,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"The behavior analyst today\",\"volume\":\"9 1\",\"pages\":\"27-33\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2008-01-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"46\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"The behavior analyst today\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100644\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The behavior analyst today","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100644","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 46

摘要

死灰复燃是指当最近增强的反应消失时,先前消失的反应恢复(例如,Epstein, 1983,1985)。图1使用假设数据说明了复苏。在第一种情况下,只有反应A得到了强化。反应A在第二种情况下消失,同时反应B得到强化。反应B在第三种情况下消失,随后反应A的恢复是死灰复燃。反应A的恢复程度可以与从未加强的不同(对照)反应(反应C)进行比较。在第三种情况下测量反应C可以作为灭绝引起的反应变异性的测量(例如,Antonitis, 1951)。因此,死灰复燃提供了一种更精确的灭绝诱导行为特征。也就是说,预测在灭绝中发生的反应是那些先前在可比刺激条件下得到加强的反应。虽然在20世纪70年代研究了复苏和相关的灭绝现象(例如,Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976;Pacitti, & Smith, 1977;Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977),以及更早的(见Epstein, 1985), Epstein(1983, 1985)通常被认为是关于理解复苏的决定因素和后果的效用的最有力的评论员(另见Epstein & Skinner, 1980)。爱泼斯坦认为,这种复苏为行为分析师提供了一种更具描述性的方法来理解有时归因于弗洛伊德回归概念的发现。对弗洛伊德来说,回归是指个体回归到在个体发展早期被认为是可接受的先前表现的行为。除了将复苏和回归联系起来之外,Epstein还总结了许多与复苏相关的实验发现。因此,本文的目的是描述与复苏有关的最新实验发现。通过展示该主题与行为分析内外的各种主题(行为史,药物复发,严重问题行为,沟通障碍和认知)的相关性,本文敦促基础和应用行为分析师继续分析复苏。诉诸于生物体的行为史通常为行为分析家提供了解释复杂行为的手段(例如,Barrett, 1986;Doughty, Cirino, Mayfield, da Silva, Okouchi, & Lattal, 2005;Lattal, & Neef, 1996;Tatham, & Wanchisen, 1998;Wanchisen, & Tatham, 1991)。广义地说,复苏是一种行为历史效应,因为当前的行为只能通过诉诸于生物体先前暴露的强化的偶然性来理解。因此,复兴研究中的一个主要问题是:从过去的经验和当前的环境条件来看,产生复兴的必要和充分条件是什么?living和latal(2003)通过几个实验解决了上面提出的问题。在一项实验中,在第一种情况下,鸽子的啄键行为通过传递谷物得到加强。在第二种情况下,键啄被消灭,同时踏板压的加强(也通过粮食输送)。在第三种情况下,停止踩踏板,并测量啄键的恢复情况。通过重复这三个条件,并在第三个条件下持续观察键击死灰复燃,证实了死灰复燃是一个普遍和可复制的发现。在随后的两个实验中,研究了在产生按键回火过程中灭火踩压的必要性。在一个实验中,在第三种情况下不安排灭绝,而是使送餐与反应无关。在这种情况下,啄键现象不仅没有出现,而且在随后的情况下,当灭绝取代了与反应无关的食物递送计划时,啄键现象又出现了。…
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Extinction-Induced Response Resurgence: A Selective Review
Resurgence refers to the recovery of previously extinguished responding when a recently reinforced response is extinguished (e.g., Epstein, 1983, 1985). Figure 1 illustrates resurgence using hypothetical data. In the first condition, only Response A is reinforced. Response A is extinguished in the second condition concurrent with the reinforcement of Response B. Response B is extinguished in the third condition, and the subsequent recovery of Response A is resurgence. The degree of recovery of Response A can be compared to a different (control) response that never was reinforced (Response C). Measuring Response C in this third condition serves as a measure of extinction-induced response variability (e.g., Antonitis, 1951). Thus, resurgence provides a more refined characterization of extinction-induced behavior. That is, responses predicted to occur in extinction are those responses that previously were reinforced under comparable stimulus conditions. Although resurgence and related extinction-induced phenomena were investigated in the 1970s (e.g., Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976; Pacitti, & Smith, 1977; Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977), as well as earlier (see Epstein, 1985), Epstein (1983, 1985) usually is viewed as being the most forceful commentator regarding the utility of understanding the determinants and consequences of resurgence (see also Epstein & Skinner, 1980). Epstein argued that resurgence provides behavior analysts with a more descriptive means of understanding findings sometimes attributed to Freud's concept of regression. For Freud, regression involved an individual regressing to previously demonstrated behavior considered acceptable earlier in the individual's development. In addition to linking resurgence and regression, Epstein also summarized much of the experimental findings related to resurgence. The aim of the present paper, therefore, is to describe more recent experimental findings related to resurgence. By showing the relevance of the topic to a variety of topics in and outside behavior analysis (behavioral history, drug relapse, severe problem behavior, communication disorders, and cognition), this paper urges both basic and applied behavior analysts to continue the analysis of resurgence. Behavioral history Appealing to the behavioral history of an organism often provides behavior analysts with a means of explaining complex behavior (e.g., Barrett, 1986; Doughty, Cirino, Mayfield, da Silva, Okouchi, & Lattal, 2005; Lattal, & Neef, 1996; Tatham, & Wanchisen, 1998; Wanchisen, & Tatham, 1991). Broadly speaking, resurgence is a behavioral-history effect in that current behavior only can be understood by appealing to contingencies of reinforcement exposed to the organism previously. A primary issue, therefore, in the study of resurgence is: what are the necessary and sufficient conditions to produce resurgence, in terms of both past experiences and current environmental conditions? Lieving and Lattal (2003) addressed the question posed above across several experiments. In one experiment, key pecking of pigeons was reinforced via grain delivery in the first condition. In the second condition, key pecking was extinguished concurrent with the reinforcement of treadle pressing (also via grain delivery). In the third condition, treadle pressing was extinguished, and the resurgence of key pecking was measured. By repeating these three conditions , and consistently observing key-peck resurgence in the third condition, it was confirmed that resurgence is a general and replicable finding. In two subsequent experiments, the necessity of extinguishing treadle pressing in producing key-peck resurgence was investigated. In one experiment, instead of arranging extinction in the third condition, food delivery was made response independent. Not only was key-peck resurgence absent in this condition, but in a subsequent condition key pecking resurged when extinction replaced the response-independent, food-delivery schedule. …
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信