奥托·科恩伯格《神经生物学新发展对精神分析对象关系理论的启示》述评

Q3 Psychology
N. McWilliams
{"title":"奥托·科恩伯格《神经生物学新发展对精神分析对象关系理论的启示》述评","authors":"N. McWilliams","doi":"10.1080/15294145.2022.2054019","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Freud famously hoped that once brain-research techniques had advanced significantly, neuroscientific concepts would replace his hypothetical constructs and metaphors for the human psyche. In 1914, for example, he stated that “all our provisional ideas in psychology will presumably some day be based on an organic substructure” consisting of “special substances and chemical processes” (Freud, 1914, p. 78). Research on the brain is now sufficiently advanced that such a day may be quickly approaching. Otto Kernberg has greeted that research warmly here, with this thoughtful consideration of the implications of contemporary neuroscience for psychoanalytic theories and treatments. For at least three reasons, Kernberg is an excellent person to address this question. First, he is temperamentally integrative, as evidenced by the fact that at a time when ego psychology and object relations theories were rigidly polarized, he synthesized them in ways that liberated psychoanalysis from an increasingly stale controversy (e.g., Kernberg, 1976). He has integrated scholarship in personality with literatures on group and organizational psychology (e.g., Kernberg, 1998). And he has persistently urged psychoanalytic institutes to expand their curricula to include material from academic disciplines outside the usual canon (e.g., Kernberg & Michels, 2016). His knowledge of relevant theoretical and empirical literatures is encyclopedic. Second, Kernberg’s thinking is original, independent, and skeptical of conventional orthodoxies. Despite the resistance of some colleagues to diluting the “pure gold” of “classical” analytic technique, he has developed innovative methods for treating borderline conditions and severe personality disorders (e.g., Caligor et al., 2018; Kernberg, 1975, 1984). He has recurrently critiqued conventional models of analytic training (e.g., Kernberg, 1986, 1996, 2000, 2014). In contrast to analysts who insist that psychodynamic artistry does not lend itself to scientific investigation, he has conscientiously subjected his own innovations to empirical scrutiny (e.g., Levy et al., 2006). When scientific study has controverted his beliefs, he has taken the unusual step of publicly recanting them (e.g., Kernberg, 2003). Third, and most relevant here, Kernberg feels strongly about establishing and maintaining the scientific status of psychoanalytic theories, especially theories about technique for serious psychopathology. In this essay, he integrates current neuroscience with his decades of clinical experience, empirical investigation, and theorizing. He reviews evidence for affects as primary motivational systems, investigates the neurobiology of the child’s emerging awareness of self and other, confronts the controversial notion of a death drive, and summarizes the evolution of the dynamic unconscious. In the process, while finding support for some Freudian ideas, he challenges and suggests revisions to others. Finally, he explicates in both neuroscientific and psychoanalytic languages the earliest phases of human mental development, commenting on the personality consequences when epigenetic factors and traumatic experiences arrest a child’s maturation at each stage. From this summary, he derives implications for psychotherapy. To therapists, probably the most salient conclusions Kernberg has drawn from neuroscience involve the primacy of affects and the inference that what analysts have called “drives” are secondary phenomena, built on constellations of affects that have been sorted, as the infant matures, into positive and negative categories. This formulation follows arguments by Solms (2021) and others (e.g., Damasio, 1994) for the origin in","PeriodicalId":39493,"journal":{"name":"Neuropsychoanalysis","volume":"24 1","pages":"43 - 46"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2022-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"1","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Comments on Otto Kernberg’s “Some Implications of New Developments in Neurobiology for Psychoanalytic Object Relations Theory”\",\"authors\":\"N. McWilliams\",\"doi\":\"10.1080/15294145.2022.2054019\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Freud famously hoped that once brain-research techniques had advanced significantly, neuroscientific concepts would replace his hypothetical constructs and metaphors for the human psyche. In 1914, for example, he stated that “all our provisional ideas in psychology will presumably some day be based on an organic substructure” consisting of “special substances and chemical processes” (Freud, 1914, p. 78). Research on the brain is now sufficiently advanced that such a day may be quickly approaching. Otto Kernberg has greeted that research warmly here, with this thoughtful consideration of the implications of contemporary neuroscience for psychoanalytic theories and treatments. For at least three reasons, Kernberg is an excellent person to address this question. First, he is temperamentally integrative, as evidenced by the fact that at a time when ego psychology and object relations theories were rigidly polarized, he synthesized them in ways that liberated psychoanalysis from an increasingly stale controversy (e.g., Kernberg, 1976). He has integrated scholarship in personality with literatures on group and organizational psychology (e.g., Kernberg, 1998). And he has persistently urged psychoanalytic institutes to expand their curricula to include material from academic disciplines outside the usual canon (e.g., Kernberg & Michels, 2016). His knowledge of relevant theoretical and empirical literatures is encyclopedic. Second, Kernberg’s thinking is original, independent, and skeptical of conventional orthodoxies. Despite the resistance of some colleagues to diluting the “pure gold” of “classical” analytic technique, he has developed innovative methods for treating borderline conditions and severe personality disorders (e.g., Caligor et al., 2018; Kernberg, 1975, 1984). He has recurrently critiqued conventional models of analytic training (e.g., Kernberg, 1986, 1996, 2000, 2014). In contrast to analysts who insist that psychodynamic artistry does not lend itself to scientific investigation, he has conscientiously subjected his own innovations to empirical scrutiny (e.g., Levy et al., 2006). When scientific study has controverted his beliefs, he has taken the unusual step of publicly recanting them (e.g., Kernberg, 2003). Third, and most relevant here, Kernberg feels strongly about establishing and maintaining the scientific status of psychoanalytic theories, especially theories about technique for serious psychopathology. In this essay, he integrates current neuroscience with his decades of clinical experience, empirical investigation, and theorizing. He reviews evidence for affects as primary motivational systems, investigates the neurobiology of the child’s emerging awareness of self and other, confronts the controversial notion of a death drive, and summarizes the evolution of the dynamic unconscious. In the process, while finding support for some Freudian ideas, he challenges and suggests revisions to others. Finally, he explicates in both neuroscientific and psychoanalytic languages the earliest phases of human mental development, commenting on the personality consequences when epigenetic factors and traumatic experiences arrest a child’s maturation at each stage. From this summary, he derives implications for psychotherapy. To therapists, probably the most salient conclusions Kernberg has drawn from neuroscience involve the primacy of affects and the inference that what analysts have called “drives” are secondary phenomena, built on constellations of affects that have been sorted, as the infant matures, into positive and negative categories. This formulation follows arguments by Solms (2021) and others (e.g., Damasio, 1994) for the origin in\",\"PeriodicalId\":39493,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Neuropsychoanalysis\",\"volume\":\"24 1\",\"pages\":\"43 - 46\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2022-01-02\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"1\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Neuropsychoanalysis\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1080/15294145.2022.2054019\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"Psychology\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Neuropsychoanalysis","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15294145.2022.2054019","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"Psychology","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

摘要

弗洛伊德著名地希望,一旦大脑研究技术取得重大进展,神经科学概念将取代他对人类心理的假设结构和隐喻。例如,在1914年,他指出“我们在心理学中的所有临时想法可能总有一天会建立在一个由“特殊物质和化学过程”组成的有机亚结构之上”(弗洛伊德,1914,第78页)。现在对大脑的研究已经足够深入,这样的一天可能很快就会到来。Otto Kernberg对这项研究表示热烈欢迎,他对当代神经科学对精神分析理论和治疗的影响进行了深思熟虑的思考。至少有三个原因,克恩伯格是解决这个问题的优秀人选。首先,他在气质上是一体的,这一事实证明,在自我心理学和客体关系理论被严格两极分化的时候,他将它们综合在一起,将精神分析从日益陈旧的争论中解放出来(例如,Kernberg,1976)。他将人格研究与群体和组织心理学文献相结合(例如,Kernberg,1998)。他一直敦促精神分析研究所扩大课程范围,将通常经典之外的学术学科的材料包括在内(例如,Kernberg&Michels,2016)。他对相关理论和实证文献的了解是百科全书式的。其次,克恩伯格的思想是独创的、独立的,对传统正统持怀疑态度。尽管一些同事反对稀释“经典”分析技术的“纯金”,但他开发了治疗边缘条件和严重人格障碍的创新方法(例如,Caligor等人,2018;克恩伯格,19751984)。他反复批评传统的分析训练模型(例如,Kernberg,1986、1996、2000、2014)。与那些坚持认为心理动力学艺术不适合科学研究的分析师不同,他认真地对自己的创新进行了实证审查(例如,Levy等人,2006年)。当科学研究对他的信仰提出质疑时,他采取了不同寻常的步骤,公开放弃了这些信仰(例如,Kernberg,2003)。第三,也是最相关的一点,克恩伯格强烈主张建立和保持精神分析理论的科学地位,尤其是关于严重精神病理学技术的理论。在这篇文章中,他将当前的神经科学与他几十年的临床经验、实证研究和理论相结合。他回顾了情感作为主要动机系统的证据,调查了儿童对自我和他人的新意识的神经生物学,直面了有争议的死亡驱动概念,并总结了动态无意识的进化。在这个过程中,他在为弗洛伊德的一些观点寻找支持的同时,挑战并建议对其他观点进行修改。最后,他用神经科学和精神分析的语言阐述了人类心理发展的最早阶段,并评论了当表观遗传因素和创伤经历阻碍孩子在每个阶段成熟时的人格后果。从这个总结中,他得出了心理治疗的启示。对于治疗师来说,克恩伯格从神经科学中得出的最显著的结论可能涉及情感的首要地位,以及分析人士所说的“驱动力”是次要现象的推断,这些现象建立在情感的星座之上,随着婴儿的成熟,这些情感被分为积极和消极两类。这一表述遵循了Solms(2021)和其他人(例如,Damasio,1994)关于
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Comments on Otto Kernberg’s “Some Implications of New Developments in Neurobiology for Psychoanalytic Object Relations Theory”
Freud famously hoped that once brain-research techniques had advanced significantly, neuroscientific concepts would replace his hypothetical constructs and metaphors for the human psyche. In 1914, for example, he stated that “all our provisional ideas in psychology will presumably some day be based on an organic substructure” consisting of “special substances and chemical processes” (Freud, 1914, p. 78). Research on the brain is now sufficiently advanced that such a day may be quickly approaching. Otto Kernberg has greeted that research warmly here, with this thoughtful consideration of the implications of contemporary neuroscience for psychoanalytic theories and treatments. For at least three reasons, Kernberg is an excellent person to address this question. First, he is temperamentally integrative, as evidenced by the fact that at a time when ego psychology and object relations theories were rigidly polarized, he synthesized them in ways that liberated psychoanalysis from an increasingly stale controversy (e.g., Kernberg, 1976). He has integrated scholarship in personality with literatures on group and organizational psychology (e.g., Kernberg, 1998). And he has persistently urged psychoanalytic institutes to expand their curricula to include material from academic disciplines outside the usual canon (e.g., Kernberg & Michels, 2016). His knowledge of relevant theoretical and empirical literatures is encyclopedic. Second, Kernberg’s thinking is original, independent, and skeptical of conventional orthodoxies. Despite the resistance of some colleagues to diluting the “pure gold” of “classical” analytic technique, he has developed innovative methods for treating borderline conditions and severe personality disorders (e.g., Caligor et al., 2018; Kernberg, 1975, 1984). He has recurrently critiqued conventional models of analytic training (e.g., Kernberg, 1986, 1996, 2000, 2014). In contrast to analysts who insist that psychodynamic artistry does not lend itself to scientific investigation, he has conscientiously subjected his own innovations to empirical scrutiny (e.g., Levy et al., 2006). When scientific study has controverted his beliefs, he has taken the unusual step of publicly recanting them (e.g., Kernberg, 2003). Third, and most relevant here, Kernberg feels strongly about establishing and maintaining the scientific status of psychoanalytic theories, especially theories about technique for serious psychopathology. In this essay, he integrates current neuroscience with his decades of clinical experience, empirical investigation, and theorizing. He reviews evidence for affects as primary motivational systems, investigates the neurobiology of the child’s emerging awareness of self and other, confronts the controversial notion of a death drive, and summarizes the evolution of the dynamic unconscious. In the process, while finding support for some Freudian ideas, he challenges and suggests revisions to others. Finally, he explicates in both neuroscientific and psychoanalytic languages the earliest phases of human mental development, commenting on the personality consequences when epigenetic factors and traumatic experiences arrest a child’s maturation at each stage. From this summary, he derives implications for psychotherapy. To therapists, probably the most salient conclusions Kernberg has drawn from neuroscience involve the primacy of affects and the inference that what analysts have called “drives” are secondary phenomena, built on constellations of affects that have been sorted, as the infant matures, into positive and negative categories. This formulation follows arguments by Solms (2021) and others (e.g., Damasio, 1994) for the origin in
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Neuropsychoanalysis
Neuropsychoanalysis Psychology-Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology
CiteScore
2.50
自引率
0.00%
发文量
24
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信