对目睹虐待性头部创伤的评论的回应:事故显示颅内病变的发生率高于摇晃

Chris B. Brook
{"title":"对目睹虐待性头部创伤的评论的回应:事故显示颅内病变的发生率高于摇晃","authors":"Chris B. Brook","doi":"10.1002/cns3.70018","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>I thank Drs. Greeley and Anderst for taking the time to comment1 on my article “Witnessing abusive head trauma: Accidents show higher rates of intracranial pathologies than shaking.”2</p><p>The authors raise issues regarding the validity of comparing different data sets. They claim that “pediBIRN is a prospective, cohort study.” In fact, pediBIRN starts with an outcome (child with brain injury), then looks backward (using a clinician's judgment) to decide exposure (accident versus abuse). The PediBIRN authors themselves refer to the data as “strictly observational.”3 So whilst the registry of head injury cases is prosepective, it is <i>not</i> a prospective cohort study of exposures. The studies used in my article all have directionality of outcome to exposure.</p><p>Further, the base rate for shaking in my study is “witnessed shaking events,” regardless of which of the four studies4 the data were drawn from. I compiled a list of witnessed cases of shaking from the literature, and the number of such witnessed cases became the base rate in my study. I have not incorporated different base rates from the different studies into my study.</p><p>Nevertheless, the authors raise a central, crucial point regarding selection bias. Abusive head trauma (AHT) encompasses a spectrum—from a slap to violent shaking to forceful impact—just as accidental injury ranges from minor bumps to severe falls. Comparing rates of findings in all AHT cases versus all accidents is intractable. Any comparison of rates will be subject to selection bias, particularly selections that influence how severe was the accident and how severe was the abuse.</p><p>This is demonstrated in my previous article, “Retino-dural hemorrhages in infants are markers of degree of intracranial pathology not of violent shaking,”5 which shows that when studies claim certain findings are more common in AHT (i.e., specific for AHT), this apparent specificity stems from selection bias. Such studies typically compare severe cases of AHT with less severe accidental injuries. By contrast, when cases are matched for severity (as reflected by hypoxic brain injury), the findings supposedly associated with AHT instead correlate with the degree of brain injury—independent of cause or intent.</p><p>The other problem with articles that claim certain findings to be specific to AHT is the methods used for classification of cases as AHT, which are biased towards selecting cases with certain pathologies, creating a self-fulfilling prophecy. The motivation for my article was to restrict comparisons to cases that were witnessed, providing a more robust classification that does not suffer from such circular reasoning.</p><p>Unfortunately there is a paucity of such data, and my study required assembling data from different sources. This was explained as a limitation of the study, so I agree with the authors that a degree of caution is warranted in assessing the quoted rates, and I once again reiterate that it will be very helpful to repeat the study with a single high-quality data set.</p><p>How then should we interpret these findings from witnessed cases of accidents and shaking? The data clearly show that shaking incidents rarely cause serious injury and typically do not produce the intracranial or ocular findings classically associated with inertial forces or AHT. Indeed, not once has an independently witnessed or videotaped violent shaking of a healthy infant <i>ever</i> resulted in the findings associated with AHT. By contrast, the data shows that independently witnessed accidents (and even videotaped short falls) can cause such findings, even if it is clear that most accidents will not.</p><p>It is becoming increasingly clear that results showing that certain findings are specific to AHT, to shaking, or to inertial forces are only found in studies that use circular reasoning6 and/or incorporation bias, and that such results are not reproduced in studies that do not use such deeply flawed methodologies.</p><p><b>Chris B. Brook:</b> conceptualization, writing – original draft.</p>","PeriodicalId":72232,"journal":{"name":"Annals of the Child Neurology Society","volume":"3 3","pages":"255-256"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2025-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/cns3.70018","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Response to Comment on Witnessing Abusive Head Trauma: Accidents Show Higher Rates of Intracranial Pathologies Than Shaking\",\"authors\":\"Chris B. Brook\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/cns3.70018\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>I thank Drs. Greeley and Anderst for taking the time to comment1 on my article “Witnessing abusive head trauma: Accidents show higher rates of intracranial pathologies than shaking.”2</p><p>The authors raise issues regarding the validity of comparing different data sets. They claim that “pediBIRN is a prospective, cohort study.” In fact, pediBIRN starts with an outcome (child with brain injury), then looks backward (using a clinician's judgment) to decide exposure (accident versus abuse). The PediBIRN authors themselves refer to the data as “strictly observational.”3 So whilst the registry of head injury cases is prosepective, it is <i>not</i> a prospective cohort study of exposures. The studies used in my article all have directionality of outcome to exposure.</p><p>Further, the base rate for shaking in my study is “witnessed shaking events,” regardless of which of the four studies4 the data were drawn from. I compiled a list of witnessed cases of shaking from the literature, and the number of such witnessed cases became the base rate in my study. I have not incorporated different base rates from the different studies into my study.</p><p>Nevertheless, the authors raise a central, crucial point regarding selection bias. Abusive head trauma (AHT) encompasses a spectrum—from a slap to violent shaking to forceful impact—just as accidental injury ranges from minor bumps to severe falls. Comparing rates of findings in all AHT cases versus all accidents is intractable. Any comparison of rates will be subject to selection bias, particularly selections that influence how severe was the accident and how severe was the abuse.</p><p>This is demonstrated in my previous article, “Retino-dural hemorrhages in infants are markers of degree of intracranial pathology not of violent shaking,”5 which shows that when studies claim certain findings are more common in AHT (i.e., specific for AHT), this apparent specificity stems from selection bias. Such studies typically compare severe cases of AHT with less severe accidental injuries. By contrast, when cases are matched for severity (as reflected by hypoxic brain injury), the findings supposedly associated with AHT instead correlate with the degree of brain injury—independent of cause or intent.</p><p>The other problem with articles that claim certain findings to be specific to AHT is the methods used for classification of cases as AHT, which are biased towards selecting cases with certain pathologies, creating a self-fulfilling prophecy. The motivation for my article was to restrict comparisons to cases that were witnessed, providing a more robust classification that does not suffer from such circular reasoning.</p><p>Unfortunately there is a paucity of such data, and my study required assembling data from different sources. This was explained as a limitation of the study, so I agree with the authors that a degree of caution is warranted in assessing the quoted rates, and I once again reiterate that it will be very helpful to repeat the study with a single high-quality data set.</p><p>How then should we interpret these findings from witnessed cases of accidents and shaking? The data clearly show that shaking incidents rarely cause serious injury and typically do not produce the intracranial or ocular findings classically associated with inertial forces or AHT. Indeed, not once has an independently witnessed or videotaped violent shaking of a healthy infant <i>ever</i> resulted in the findings associated with AHT. By contrast, the data shows that independently witnessed accidents (and even videotaped short falls) can cause such findings, even if it is clear that most accidents will not.</p><p>It is becoming increasingly clear that results showing that certain findings are specific to AHT, to shaking, or to inertial forces are only found in studies that use circular reasoning6 and/or incorporation bias, and that such results are not reproduced in studies that do not use such deeply flawed methodologies.</p><p><b>Chris B. Brook:</b> conceptualization, writing – original draft.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":72232,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Annals of the Child Neurology Society\",\"volume\":\"3 3\",\"pages\":\"255-256\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-08-04\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/cns3.70018\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Annals of the Child Neurology Society\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cns3.70018\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Annals of the Child Neurology Society","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cns3.70018","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

我感谢dr。格里利和安德斯特花时间评论了我的文章《目睹虐待性头部创伤:意外事故显示颅内病变的发生率高于摇晃》。作者提出了关于比较不同数据集的有效性的问题。他们声称“pediBIRN是一项前瞻性的队列研究。”事实上,pediBIRN从结果(儿童脑损伤)开始,然后回顾(使用临床医生的判断)来决定暴露(意外还是虐待)。PediBIRN的作者自己将这些数据称为“严格的观察数据”。“因此,虽然头部损伤病例的登记是前瞻性的,但它不是一个前瞻性的暴露队列研究。我文章中使用的研究都有暴露结果的方向性。此外,在我的研究中,震动的基本率是“目击震动事件”,而不管数据是从四项研究中的哪一项中提取的。我从文献中整理了一份震颤的目击案例列表,这些目击案例的数量成为我研究的基准比率。我没有将不同研究的不同基础率纳入我的研究。然而,作者提出了一个关于选择偏差的核心、关键的观点。虐待性头部创伤(AHT)涵盖了一系列范围——从拍打到剧烈摇晃再到强力撞击——就像意外伤害从轻微的碰撞到严重的跌落。比较所有AHT病例与所有事故的发现率是难以解决的。任何比率的比较都会受到选择偏差的影响,特别是影响事故严重程度和虐待严重程度的选择。这在我之前的文章“婴儿视网膜硬膜出血是颅内病理程度的标志,而不是剧烈摇晃的标志”中得到了证明,这表明当研究声称某些发现在AHT中更常见(即AHT特异性)时,这种明显的特异性源于选择偏差。此类研究通常将严重的AHT病例与不太严重的意外伤害进行比较。相比之下,当病例的严重程度相匹配时(如缺氧脑损伤所反映的),这些发现被认为与AHT相关,而不是与原因或意图相关的脑损伤程度。声称某些发现是AHT特有的文章的另一个问题是用于将病例分类为AHT的方法,这些方法偏向于选择具有某些病理的病例,创造了一种自我实现的预言。我写这篇文章的动机是将比较限制在亲眼目睹的案例上,提供一个更可靠的分类,而不会受到这种循环推理的影响。不幸的是,这样的数据很少,我的研究需要收集来自不同来源的数据。这被解释为研究的局限性,所以我同意作者的观点,在评估引用率时一定程度的谨慎是有必要的,我再次重申,使用单一的高质量数据集重复研究将非常有帮助。那么,我们应该如何根据亲眼目睹的事故和摇晃的案例来解释这些发现呢?数据清楚地表明,震动事件很少造成严重伤害,通常不会产生与惯性力或AHT相关的颅内或眼部症状。事实上,没有一次独立目击或录像的健康婴儿的剧烈摇晃导致与AHT相关的发现。相比之下,数据显示,独立目击的事故(甚至录像的短暂跌落)可能会导致这样的结果,即使很明显大多数事故不会。越来越清楚的是,只有在使用循环推理和(或)合并偏差的研究中,才会发现某些研究结果是AHT、震动或惯性力所特有的,而在不使用这种有严重缺陷的方法的研究中,这些结果是无法重现的。克里斯B.布鲁克:概念化,写作-原始草案。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Response to Comment on Witnessing Abusive Head Trauma: Accidents Show Higher Rates of Intracranial Pathologies Than Shaking

I thank Drs. Greeley and Anderst for taking the time to comment1 on my article “Witnessing abusive head trauma: Accidents show higher rates of intracranial pathologies than shaking.”2

The authors raise issues regarding the validity of comparing different data sets. They claim that “pediBIRN is a prospective, cohort study.” In fact, pediBIRN starts with an outcome (child with brain injury), then looks backward (using a clinician's judgment) to decide exposure (accident versus abuse). The PediBIRN authors themselves refer to the data as “strictly observational.”3 So whilst the registry of head injury cases is prosepective, it is not a prospective cohort study of exposures. The studies used in my article all have directionality of outcome to exposure.

Further, the base rate for shaking in my study is “witnessed shaking events,” regardless of which of the four studies4 the data were drawn from. I compiled a list of witnessed cases of shaking from the literature, and the number of such witnessed cases became the base rate in my study. I have not incorporated different base rates from the different studies into my study.

Nevertheless, the authors raise a central, crucial point regarding selection bias. Abusive head trauma (AHT) encompasses a spectrum—from a slap to violent shaking to forceful impact—just as accidental injury ranges from minor bumps to severe falls. Comparing rates of findings in all AHT cases versus all accidents is intractable. Any comparison of rates will be subject to selection bias, particularly selections that influence how severe was the accident and how severe was the abuse.

This is demonstrated in my previous article, “Retino-dural hemorrhages in infants are markers of degree of intracranial pathology not of violent shaking,”5 which shows that when studies claim certain findings are more common in AHT (i.e., specific for AHT), this apparent specificity stems from selection bias. Such studies typically compare severe cases of AHT with less severe accidental injuries. By contrast, when cases are matched for severity (as reflected by hypoxic brain injury), the findings supposedly associated with AHT instead correlate with the degree of brain injury—independent of cause or intent.

The other problem with articles that claim certain findings to be specific to AHT is the methods used for classification of cases as AHT, which are biased towards selecting cases with certain pathologies, creating a self-fulfilling prophecy. The motivation for my article was to restrict comparisons to cases that were witnessed, providing a more robust classification that does not suffer from such circular reasoning.

Unfortunately there is a paucity of such data, and my study required assembling data from different sources. This was explained as a limitation of the study, so I agree with the authors that a degree of caution is warranted in assessing the quoted rates, and I once again reiterate that it will be very helpful to repeat the study with a single high-quality data set.

How then should we interpret these findings from witnessed cases of accidents and shaking? The data clearly show that shaking incidents rarely cause serious injury and typically do not produce the intracranial or ocular findings classically associated with inertial forces or AHT. Indeed, not once has an independently witnessed or videotaped violent shaking of a healthy infant ever resulted in the findings associated with AHT. By contrast, the data shows that independently witnessed accidents (and even videotaped short falls) can cause such findings, even if it is clear that most accidents will not.

It is becoming increasingly clear that results showing that certain findings are specific to AHT, to shaking, or to inertial forces are only found in studies that use circular reasoning6 and/or incorporation bias, and that such results are not reproduced in studies that do not use such deeply flawed methodologies.

Chris B. Brook: conceptualization, writing – original draft.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信