有尾母羊和断尾母羊及其羔羊的母儿关系

IF 2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE
Jocelyn M. Woods, Sarah J.J. Adcock
{"title":"有尾母羊和断尾母羊及其羔羊的母儿关系","authors":"Jocelyn M. Woods,&nbsp;Sarah J.J. Adcock","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2025.106775","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Maternal behavior in the hours after lambing is critical for establishing the lamb–dam bond. In rodents, early-life pain alters maternal behavior, but it is unclear whether routine painful procedures like tail docking have similar long-term effects in sheep. We tested whether neonatal tail docking in female lambs altered (1) their maternal behavior and their offspring’s behavior after lambing and (2) behavioral and physiological stress responses to separation. Polypay ewe lambs were tail docked using the rubber ring method between 24 and 36 h of age (n = 11) or left undocked (n = 10). At approximately 13 months of age, they gave birth to singleton (n = 6) or twin (n = 15) litters. Between 30 and 60 min after the last lamb’s birth, the ewe and her lamb(s) were moved to an individual maternity pen, where behaviors were video recorded for 2 h. Between 6 and 12 h after lambing, the lambs were separated from their dam for 15 min, with video recordings taken from 15 min before separation until 15 min after reunion. Eye temperatures of ewes and lambs were assessed using infrared thermography immediately before and after separation as indicators of physiological stress. For all video recordings, we analyzed the duration the ewe spent eating, pacing, lying, and grooming her lamb(s), as well as the duration the lambs spent wagging their tail, nursing, and lying. Transitions from lying to standing were also recorded for ewes and lambs in the 2-hour postnatal period. All models included the dam’s tail docking status as a fixed effect with litter size (singleton vs twin) as a factor covariate; separation models also included observation period (before, during, and after separation). We found no significant effect of the dam’s tail docking status on maternal and offspring behavior following lambing or on their responses to separation. However, lambs of undocked ewes tended to spend more time lying in the postnatal period compared to lambs of docked ewes. Behavioral changes during and after separation – including increased pacing and reduced eating in ewes during separation, and increased grooming and nursing following reunion – suggest that separation was stressful, regardless of the ewe’s tail docking status. No differences were observed in eye temperatures before and after separation. Overall, we did not find evidence to suggest that neonatal tail docking impairs the ewe’s ability to bond with or care for her lambs later in life.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"292 ","pages":"Article 106775"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0000,"publicationDate":"2025-08-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"The maternal-offspring relationship in tailed and docked ewes and their lambs\",\"authors\":\"Jocelyn M. Woods,&nbsp;Sarah J.J. Adcock\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.applanim.2025.106775\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><div>Maternal behavior in the hours after lambing is critical for establishing the lamb–dam bond. In rodents, early-life pain alters maternal behavior, but it is unclear whether routine painful procedures like tail docking have similar long-term effects in sheep. We tested whether neonatal tail docking in female lambs altered (1) their maternal behavior and their offspring’s behavior after lambing and (2) behavioral and physiological stress responses to separation. Polypay ewe lambs were tail docked using the rubber ring method between 24 and 36 h of age (n = 11) or left undocked (n = 10). At approximately 13 months of age, they gave birth to singleton (n = 6) or twin (n = 15) litters. Between 30 and 60 min after the last lamb’s birth, the ewe and her lamb(s) were moved to an individual maternity pen, where behaviors were video recorded for 2 h. Between 6 and 12 h after lambing, the lambs were separated from their dam for 15 min, with video recordings taken from 15 min before separation until 15 min after reunion. Eye temperatures of ewes and lambs were assessed using infrared thermography immediately before and after separation as indicators of physiological stress. For all video recordings, we analyzed the duration the ewe spent eating, pacing, lying, and grooming her lamb(s), as well as the duration the lambs spent wagging their tail, nursing, and lying. Transitions from lying to standing were also recorded for ewes and lambs in the 2-hour postnatal period. All models included the dam’s tail docking status as a fixed effect with litter size (singleton vs twin) as a factor covariate; separation models also included observation period (before, during, and after separation). We found no significant effect of the dam’s tail docking status on maternal and offspring behavior following lambing or on their responses to separation. However, lambs of undocked ewes tended to spend more time lying in the postnatal period compared to lambs of docked ewes. Behavioral changes during and after separation – including increased pacing and reduced eating in ewes during separation, and increased grooming and nursing following reunion – suggest that separation was stressful, regardless of the ewe’s tail docking status. No differences were observed in eye temperatures before and after separation. Overall, we did not find evidence to suggest that neonatal tail docking impairs the ewe’s ability to bond with or care for her lambs later in life.</div></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":8222,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Applied Animal Behaviour Science\",\"volume\":\"292 \",\"pages\":\"Article 106775\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2025-08-05\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Applied Animal Behaviour Science\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"97\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159125002734\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"农林科学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","FirstCategoryId":"97","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159125002734","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

母性在产羔后数小时内的行为对建立“羊坝”关系至关重要。在啮齿类动物中,早期的疼痛会改变母性行为,但目前尚不清楚像断尾这样的常规疼痛过程是否会对绵羊产生类似的长期影响。我们测试了母羊断尾是否会改变(1)母羊的母性行为及其后代在产羔后的行为;(2)分离后的行为和生理应激反应。在24 ~ 36 h龄(n = 11)之间,采用橡胶环法对多宝母羊进行尾截(n = 10)。在大约13个月大的时候,它们生下了单胎(n = 6)或双胞胎(n = 15)。在最后一只羊羔出生后的30到60 分钟内,母羊和她的羊羔被转移到一个单独的母栏,在那里的行为被录像2 小时。在产羔后的6 - 12 h之间,将羔羊与他们的水坝分开15 分钟,并从分离前15 分钟到团聚后15 分钟录制视频。采用红外热像仪测定分离前后母羊和羔羊的眼温,作为生理应激的指标。对于所有的视频记录,我们分析了母羊进食、踱步、躺着和梳理小羊的时间,以及小羊摇尾巴、哺乳和躺着的时间。还记录了母羊和羔羊在出生后2小时内从躺着到站立的转变。所有模型都将坝尾对接状态作为一个固定效应,产仔数(单胎vs双胎)作为一个协变量;分离模型还包括观察期(分离前、分离中、分离后)。我们发现,母鼠断尾状态对母鼠和子鼠产羔后的行为或对分离的反应没有显著影响。然而,与母羊的羔羊相比,未停靠母羊的羔羊在产后往往花费更多的时间躺在床上。分离期间和分离后的行为变化——包括分离期间母羊加快踱步和减少进食,团聚后增加梳理和护理——表明分离是有压力的,无论母羊的尾合状态如何。分离前后眼温无明显差异。总的来说,我们没有发现证据表明新生儿断尾会损害母羊在以后的生活中与羔羊建立联系或照顾羔羊的能力。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
The maternal-offspring relationship in tailed and docked ewes and their lambs
Maternal behavior in the hours after lambing is critical for establishing the lamb–dam bond. In rodents, early-life pain alters maternal behavior, but it is unclear whether routine painful procedures like tail docking have similar long-term effects in sheep. We tested whether neonatal tail docking in female lambs altered (1) their maternal behavior and their offspring’s behavior after lambing and (2) behavioral and physiological stress responses to separation. Polypay ewe lambs were tail docked using the rubber ring method between 24 and 36 h of age (n = 11) or left undocked (n = 10). At approximately 13 months of age, they gave birth to singleton (n = 6) or twin (n = 15) litters. Between 30 and 60 min after the last lamb’s birth, the ewe and her lamb(s) were moved to an individual maternity pen, where behaviors were video recorded for 2 h. Between 6 and 12 h after lambing, the lambs were separated from their dam for 15 min, with video recordings taken from 15 min before separation until 15 min after reunion. Eye temperatures of ewes and lambs were assessed using infrared thermography immediately before and after separation as indicators of physiological stress. For all video recordings, we analyzed the duration the ewe spent eating, pacing, lying, and grooming her lamb(s), as well as the duration the lambs spent wagging their tail, nursing, and lying. Transitions from lying to standing were also recorded for ewes and lambs in the 2-hour postnatal period. All models included the dam’s tail docking status as a fixed effect with litter size (singleton vs twin) as a factor covariate; separation models also included observation period (before, during, and after separation). We found no significant effect of the dam’s tail docking status on maternal and offspring behavior following lambing or on their responses to separation. However, lambs of undocked ewes tended to spend more time lying in the postnatal period compared to lambs of docked ewes. Behavioral changes during and after separation – including increased pacing and reduced eating in ewes during separation, and increased grooming and nursing following reunion – suggest that separation was stressful, regardless of the ewe’s tail docking status. No differences were observed in eye temperatures before and after separation. Overall, we did not find evidence to suggest that neonatal tail docking impairs the ewe’s ability to bond with or care for her lambs later in life.
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Applied Animal Behaviour Science
Applied Animal Behaviour Science 农林科学-行为科学
CiteScore
4.40
自引率
21.70%
发文量
191
审稿时长
18.1 weeks
期刊介绍: This journal publishes relevant information on the behaviour of domesticated and utilized animals. Topics covered include: -Behaviour of farm, zoo and laboratory animals in relation to animal management and welfare -Behaviour of companion animals in relation to behavioural problems, for example, in relation to the training of dogs for different purposes, in relation to behavioural problems -Studies of the behaviour of wild animals when these studies are relevant from an applied perspective, for example in relation to wildlife management, pest management or nature conservation -Methodological studies within relevant fields The principal subjects are farm, companion and laboratory animals, including, of course, poultry. The journal also deals with the following animal subjects: -Those involved in any farming system, e.g. deer, rabbits and fur-bearing animals -Those in ANY form of confinement, e.g. zoos, safari parks and other forms of display -Feral animals, and any animal species which impinge on farming operations, e.g. as causes of loss or damage -Species used for hunting, recreation etc. may also be considered as acceptable subjects in some instances -Laboratory animals, if the material relates to their behavioural requirements
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信