{"title":"鼓励学生在学习数学时解释自己的想法:心理学视角","authors":"Bethany Rittle-Johnson","doi":"10.1016/j.jmathb.2024.101192","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Children’s <em>self-explanations</em> are answers to voiced why and how questions that attempt to make sense of new information for oneself. They often are not sophisticated or generalizable. Despite this, prompting children to generate explanations often improves their learning. After providing examples of children’s explanations, this article summarizes empirical evidence for the learning benefits of prompting people to generate explanations when learning mathematics. There is strong evidence that prompting learners to explain leads to greater conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge and procedural transfer when knowledge is assessed immediately after the learning session; there is limited evidence for greater procedural transfer after a delay. Scaffolding high-quality explanations via training or structured responses, designing prompts to carefully balance attention to important content, prompting learners to explain correct information, and prompting learners to explain why incorrect information is incorrect when appropriate, increases the benefits of prompts to generate explanations.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":47481,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Mathematical Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0000,"publicationDate":"2024-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0732312324000695/pdfft?md5=da899ecf252efeddeeea91ce27ec15c7&pid=1-s2.0-S0732312324000695-main.pdf","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Encouraging students to explain their ideas when learning mathematics: A psychological perspective\",\"authors\":\"Bethany Rittle-Johnson\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.jmathb.2024.101192\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><div>Children’s <em>self-explanations</em> are answers to voiced why and how questions that attempt to make sense of new information for oneself. They often are not sophisticated or generalizable. Despite this, prompting children to generate explanations often improves their learning. After providing examples of children’s explanations, this article summarizes empirical evidence for the learning benefits of prompting people to generate explanations when learning mathematics. There is strong evidence that prompting learners to explain leads to greater conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge and procedural transfer when knowledge is assessed immediately after the learning session; there is limited evidence for greater procedural transfer after a delay. Scaffolding high-quality explanations via training or structured responses, designing prompts to carefully balance attention to important content, prompting learners to explain correct information, and prompting learners to explain why incorrect information is incorrect when appropriate, increases the benefits of prompts to generate explanations.</div></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":47481,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Journal of Mathematical Behavior\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-09-24\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0732312324000695/pdfft?md5=da899ecf252efeddeeea91ce27ec15c7&pid=1-s2.0-S0732312324000695-main.pdf\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Journal of Mathematical Behavior\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0732312324000695\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Mathematical Behavior","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0732312324000695","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH","Score":null,"Total":0}
Encouraging students to explain their ideas when learning mathematics: A psychological perspective
Children’s self-explanations are answers to voiced why and how questions that attempt to make sense of new information for oneself. They often are not sophisticated or generalizable. Despite this, prompting children to generate explanations often improves their learning. After providing examples of children’s explanations, this article summarizes empirical evidence for the learning benefits of prompting people to generate explanations when learning mathematics. There is strong evidence that prompting learners to explain leads to greater conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge and procedural transfer when knowledge is assessed immediately after the learning session; there is limited evidence for greater procedural transfer after a delay. Scaffolding high-quality explanations via training or structured responses, designing prompts to carefully balance attention to important content, prompting learners to explain correct information, and prompting learners to explain why incorrect information is incorrect when appropriate, increases the benefits of prompts to generate explanations.
期刊介绍:
The Journal of Mathematical Behavior solicits original research on the learning and teaching of mathematics. We are interested especially in basic research, research that aims to clarify, in detail and depth, how mathematical ideas develop in learners. Over three decades, our experience confirms a founding premise of this journal: that mathematical thinking, hence mathematics learning as a social enterprise, is special. It is special because mathematics is special, both logically and psychologically. Logically, through the way that mathematical ideas and methods have been built, refined and organized for centuries across a range of cultures; and psychologically, through the variety of ways people today, in many walks of life, make sense of mathematics, develop it, make it their own.