约会早期记忆:事件到底发生在什么时候?

IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL
Carole Peterson , Qi Wang , Darcy Hallett , Sophie Bartlett , Amanda Y.F. Ma , Melissa M. MacKay , Emma J. Pretty , Luciana Viscarra , Elaine Y. Wang
{"title":"约会早期记忆:事件到底发生在什么时候?","authors":"Carole Peterson ,&nbsp;Qi Wang ,&nbsp;Darcy Hallett ,&nbsp;Sophie Bartlett ,&nbsp;Amanda Y.F. Ma ,&nbsp;Melissa M. MacKay ,&nbsp;Emma J. Pretty ,&nbsp;Luciana Viscarra ,&nbsp;Elaine Y. Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Can children accurately date their early memories? This question has important real-life consequences such as when jurors evaluate the credibility of child eyewitness testimony in court. Answering this question is difficult given that adults present at remembered events may be inaccurate themselves in retroactively dating the memories recalled by their children, and often cannot provide reliable validation. In this study, prior to child interviews the parents of 6- to 13-year-olds provided eight memories of events with known dates, two each from when children were age 2, 3, 4, and 5 years. A total of 104 6- to 13-year-olds participated (47 % female, 70 % White and 26 % Asian or multi-ethnic), recruited from Canada (36 %) and USA (64 %). Children typically made systematic dating errors. Memories of events that had occurred when children had been age 2 were misdated by 1½ years on average, and as children’s age at the time of remembered events increased, misdating errors decreased. Errors usually involved children thinking they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually were – a phenomenon termed ‘forward telescoping’ (versus ‘backward telescoping, when individuals think that they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually had been). For example, many of the events from when children were age 2 were recalled by the children, but they misdated them to older ages. Although ‘age of memory’ (age of the child at the time of the remembered event) was significantly related to errors in dating, with more errors for memories from younger periods of their lives, ‘age of child’ at the time they did the memory task did not differ depending upon how old the children were. Findings have theoretical and forensic implications.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8000,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000741/pdfft?md5=937432cd47a9c31986716e10d60efb5f&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000741-main.pdf","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Dating early memories: When did events really happen?\",\"authors\":\"Carole Peterson ,&nbsp;Qi Wang ,&nbsp;Darcy Hallett ,&nbsp;Sophie Bartlett ,&nbsp;Amanda Y.F. Ma ,&nbsp;Melissa M. MacKay ,&nbsp;Emma J. Pretty ,&nbsp;Luciana Viscarra ,&nbsp;Elaine Y. Wang\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101489\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><p>Can children accurately date their early memories? This question has important real-life consequences such as when jurors evaluate the credibility of child eyewitness testimony in court. Answering this question is difficult given that adults present at remembered events may be inaccurate themselves in retroactively dating the memories recalled by their children, and often cannot provide reliable validation. In this study, prior to child interviews the parents of 6- to 13-year-olds provided eight memories of events with known dates, two each from when children were age 2, 3, 4, and 5 years. A total of 104 6- to 13-year-olds participated (47 % female, 70 % White and 26 % Asian or multi-ethnic), recruited from Canada (36 %) and USA (64 %). Children typically made systematic dating errors. Memories of events that had occurred when children had been age 2 were misdated by 1½ years on average, and as children’s age at the time of remembered events increased, misdating errors decreased. Errors usually involved children thinking they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually were – a phenomenon termed ‘forward telescoping’ (versus ‘backward telescoping, when individuals think that they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually had been). For example, many of the events from when children were age 2 were recalled by the children, but they misdated them to older ages. Although ‘age of memory’ (age of the child at the time of the remembered event) was significantly related to errors in dating, with more errors for memories from younger periods of their lives, ‘age of child’ at the time they did the memory task did not differ depending upon how old the children were. Findings have theoretical and forensic implications.</p></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":51422,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Cognitive Development\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":1.8000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-07-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000741/pdfft?md5=937432cd47a9c31986716e10d60efb5f&pid=1-s2.0-S0885201424000741-main.pdf\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Cognitive Development\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000741\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q3\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Cognitive Development","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201424000741","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

儿童能否准确地确定其早期记忆的日期?这个问题在现实生活中具有重要影响,例如陪审员在法庭上评估儿童目击证人证词的可信度时。要回答这个问题是很困难的,因为当时在场的成年人在追溯孩子的记忆时可能并不准确,而且往往无法提供可靠的验证。在本研究中,6 到 13 岁儿童的父母在对儿童进行访谈之前,提供了 8 个已知日期的事件记忆,其中 2 个分别来自儿童 2 岁、3 岁、4 岁和 5 岁时。共有 104 名 6 至 13 岁的儿童(47% 为女性,70% 为白人,26% 为亚裔或多种族)参加了此次访谈,他们分别来自加拿大(36%)和美国(64%)。儿童通常会犯系统性的约会错误。儿童对 2 岁时发生的事件的记忆平均错记了 1 年半,随着儿童记忆事件发生时年龄的增加,错记日期的错误也随之减少。错误通常涉及儿童认为他们在记忆事件发生时的年龄比实际年龄大--这种现象被称为 "向前伸缩"(相对于 "向后伸缩",即个人认为他们在记忆事件发生时的年龄比实际年龄大)。例如,孩子们回忆起了许多他们 2 岁时发生的事情,但他们却错误地把这些事情的时间推算到了更大的年龄。虽然 "记忆年龄"(记忆事件发生时儿童的年龄)与年代错误有很大关系,儿童对较年轻时期的记忆错误较多,但儿童做记忆任务时的 "儿童年龄 "并不因儿童的年龄而异。研究结果具有理论和法医学意义。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
Dating early memories: When did events really happen?

Can children accurately date their early memories? This question has important real-life consequences such as when jurors evaluate the credibility of child eyewitness testimony in court. Answering this question is difficult given that adults present at remembered events may be inaccurate themselves in retroactively dating the memories recalled by their children, and often cannot provide reliable validation. In this study, prior to child interviews the parents of 6- to 13-year-olds provided eight memories of events with known dates, two each from when children were age 2, 3, 4, and 5 years. A total of 104 6- to 13-year-olds participated (47 % female, 70 % White and 26 % Asian or multi-ethnic), recruited from Canada (36 %) and USA (64 %). Children typically made systematic dating errors. Memories of events that had occurred when children had been age 2 were misdated by 1½ years on average, and as children’s age at the time of remembered events increased, misdating errors decreased. Errors usually involved children thinking they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually were – a phenomenon termed ‘forward telescoping’ (versus ‘backward telescoping, when individuals think that they had been older at the time of remembered events than they actually had been). For example, many of the events from when children were age 2 were recalled by the children, but they misdated them to older ages. Although ‘age of memory’ (age of the child at the time of the remembered event) was significantly related to errors in dating, with more errors for memories from younger periods of their lives, ‘age of child’ at the time they did the memory task did not differ depending upon how old the children were. Findings have theoretical and forensic implications.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
CiteScore
3.20
自引率
5.60%
发文量
114
期刊介绍: Cognitive Development contains the very best empirical and theoretical work on the development of perception, memory, language, concepts, thinking, problem solving, metacognition, and social cognition. Criteria for acceptance of articles will be: significance of the work to issues of current interest, substance of the argument, and clarity of expression. For purposes of publication in Cognitive Development, moral and social development will be considered part of cognitive development when they are related to the development of knowledge or thought processes.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信