Janie Corley, Alison Pattie, Ian J. Deary, Simon R. Cox
{"title":"园艺与认知老化:1921 年洛锡安出生队列的纵向研究结果","authors":"Janie Corley, Alison Pattie, Ian J. Deary, Simon R. Cox","doi":"10.1016/j.jenvp.2024.102361","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Identifying modifiable lifestyle behaviours that facilitate healthy cognitive ageing is of major public interest for the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia. Gardening is associated with numerous health benefits, yet few studies have explored the significance of gardening activity in relation to cognitive ageing. Here, longitudinal cognitive data from 467 older adults (aged 79 ± 0.6) of the LBC1921 study were used to examine associations of self-reported gardening at age 79 with lifetime cognitive change (age 11 to 79), and cognitive level (age 79) and slope across later life (age 79 to 90). Cognitive function was assessed at age 11 and 79 using the Moray House Test (MHT), and a battery of psychometric tests assessed cognitive function at ages 79, 83, 87 and 90 (MHT, Raven's Matrices, Verbal Fluency, Logical Memory). Models controlled for individual-level socio-demographic, lifestyle and health covariates including overall physical activity. Higher frequency of gardening was significantly associated with greater lifetime cognitive gain from age 11 to 79 on the MHT (std β = 0.112 P = 0.024), higher general cognitive ability (<em>g</em>) level at age 79 (std β = 0.145, P = 0.001), and higher test-specific scores on the MHT (std β = 0.153, P = 0.001) and Raven's Matrices (std β = 0.102, P = 0.040). Gardening was not associated with subsequent cognitive slopes from age 79 to 90. Overall, these findings suggest that engaging in gardening activities may contribute modestly to healthy cognitive functioning up to the eighth decade of life. Although physical activity was higher in gardeners, it did not explain the gardening and cognitive function relationship.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48439,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Environmental Psychology","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 102361"},"PeriodicalIF":6.1000,"publicationDate":"2024-06-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Gardening and cognitive ageing: Longitudinal findings from the lothian birth cohort of 1921\",\"authors\":\"Janie Corley, Alison Pattie, Ian J. Deary, Simon R. Cox\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.jenvp.2024.102361\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><p>Identifying modifiable lifestyle behaviours that facilitate healthy cognitive ageing is of major public interest for the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia. Gardening is associated with numerous health benefits, yet few studies have explored the significance of gardening activity in relation to cognitive ageing. Here, longitudinal cognitive data from 467 older adults (aged 79 ± 0.6) of the LBC1921 study were used to examine associations of self-reported gardening at age 79 with lifetime cognitive change (age 11 to 79), and cognitive level (age 79) and slope across later life (age 79 to 90). Cognitive function was assessed at age 11 and 79 using the Moray House Test (MHT), and a battery of psychometric tests assessed cognitive function at ages 79, 83, 87 and 90 (MHT, Raven's Matrices, Verbal Fluency, Logical Memory). Models controlled for individual-level socio-demographic, lifestyle and health covariates including overall physical activity. Higher frequency of gardening was significantly associated with greater lifetime cognitive gain from age 11 to 79 on the MHT (std β = 0.112 P = 0.024), higher general cognitive ability (<em>g</em>) level at age 79 (std β = 0.145, P = 0.001), and higher test-specific scores on the MHT (std β = 0.153, P = 0.001) and Raven's Matrices (std β = 0.102, P = 0.040). Gardening was not associated with subsequent cognitive slopes from age 79 to 90. Overall, these findings suggest that engaging in gardening activities may contribute modestly to healthy cognitive functioning up to the eighth decade of life. Although physical activity was higher in gardeners, it did not explain the gardening and cognitive function relationship.</p></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":48439,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Journal of Environmental Psychology\",\"volume\":\"97 \",\"pages\":\"Article 102361\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":6.1000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-06-14\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Journal of Environmental Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0272494424001348\",\"RegionNum\":1,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Environmental Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0272494424001348","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES","Score":null,"Total":0}
Gardening and cognitive ageing: Longitudinal findings from the lothian birth cohort of 1921
Identifying modifiable lifestyle behaviours that facilitate healthy cognitive ageing is of major public interest for the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia. Gardening is associated with numerous health benefits, yet few studies have explored the significance of gardening activity in relation to cognitive ageing. Here, longitudinal cognitive data from 467 older adults (aged 79 ± 0.6) of the LBC1921 study were used to examine associations of self-reported gardening at age 79 with lifetime cognitive change (age 11 to 79), and cognitive level (age 79) and slope across later life (age 79 to 90). Cognitive function was assessed at age 11 and 79 using the Moray House Test (MHT), and a battery of psychometric tests assessed cognitive function at ages 79, 83, 87 and 90 (MHT, Raven's Matrices, Verbal Fluency, Logical Memory). Models controlled for individual-level socio-demographic, lifestyle and health covariates including overall physical activity. Higher frequency of gardening was significantly associated with greater lifetime cognitive gain from age 11 to 79 on the MHT (std β = 0.112 P = 0.024), higher general cognitive ability (g) level at age 79 (std β = 0.145, P = 0.001), and higher test-specific scores on the MHT (std β = 0.153, P = 0.001) and Raven's Matrices (std β = 0.102, P = 0.040). Gardening was not associated with subsequent cognitive slopes from age 79 to 90. Overall, these findings suggest that engaging in gardening activities may contribute modestly to healthy cognitive functioning up to the eighth decade of life. Although physical activity was higher in gardeners, it did not explain the gardening and cognitive function relationship.
期刊介绍:
The Journal of Environmental Psychology is the premier journal in the field, serving individuals in a wide range of disciplines who have an interest in the scientific study of the transactions and interrelationships between people and their surroundings (including built, social, natural and virtual environments, the use and abuse of nature and natural resources, and sustainability-related behavior). The journal publishes internationally contributed empirical studies and reviews of research on these topics that advance new insights. As an important forum for the field, the journal publishes some of the most influential papers in the discipline that reflect the scientific development of environmental psychology. Contributions on theoretical, methodological, and practical aspects of all human-environment interactions are welcome, along with innovative or interdisciplinary approaches that have a psychological emphasis. Research areas include: •Psychological and behavioral aspects of people and nature •Cognitive mapping, spatial cognition and wayfinding •Ecological consequences of human actions •Theories of place, place attachment, and place identity •Environmental risks and hazards: perception, behavior, and management •Perception and evaluation of buildings and natural landscapes •Effects of physical and natural settings on human cognition and health •Theories of proenvironmental behavior, norms, attitudes, and personality •Psychology of sustainability and climate change •Psychological aspects of resource management and crises •Social use of space: crowding, privacy, territoriality, personal space •Design of, and experiences related to, the physical aspects of workplaces, schools, residences, public buildings and public space