观察性干预对先前中性刺激强化特性习得的影响。

Jessica Singer-Dudek, R. D. Greer, Jeannine Schmelzkopf
{"title":"观察性干预对先前中性刺激强化特性习得的影响。","authors":"Jessica Singer-Dudek, R. D. Greer, Jeannine Schmelzkopf","doi":"10.1037/H0100410","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Humans can learn in one of two ways; through direct instruction, or through the observation of others (Greer, Singer-Dudek, & Gautreaux, 2006). Observational learning is the process in which an individual learns a new response/environment relation (one that was not previously in his repertoire) as a result of the observation of another individual receiving contact with the contingencies of reinforcement, punishment, and corrections of incorrect responses (Catania, 2007; Gautreaux, 2005). Greer et al. (2006) state that there are three different functions of observational learning, including 1) the emission of previously acquired operants, 2) the acquisition of new operants through observation, and 3) the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers through observation. Although observational effects on learning new operants has a growing literature (Brody, Lahey, & Combs, 1979; Egel, Richman, & Koegel, 1981; Griffen, Wolery, & Schuster, 1992; Goldstein & Mousetis, 1989; Greer, et. al, 2006; McDonald, Dixon, & Leblanc, 1986; Werts, Caldwell, & Wolery, 1996) and the effects of observation on performance has been studied extensively (Bandura, 1986; Bandura, Adams, & Beyer, 1977; Kazdin, 1973; Ollendick, Dailey, & Shapiro, 1983), the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers through observation appears to be a new type of observational learning that has only very recently been identified (Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008; Greer, Singer-Dudek, Delgado, & Oblak, 2007; Greer, Singer-Dudek, Longano, & Zrinzo, in press; O'Rourke, 2006). This new type of learning, identified as the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers by observation, can be defined as the acquisition of reinforcing properties of previously neutral stimuli as a function of observation (Greer et al., 2006; Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008). Conditioned reinforcement, which typically results from the stimulus-stimulus pairings of unconditioned or conditioned reinforcers with initially neutral stimuli that lead to the acquisition of reinforcing properties for the previously neutral stimuli (Catania, 2007; Donahoe & Palmer, 2004; Dinsmoor, 2004; Kelleher & Gollub, 1962) has been identified in the literature as important for increasing students' communities of reinforcers while decreasing stereotypy (Greer, Becker, Saxe, & Mirabella, 1985; Nuzzolo-Gomez, Leonard, Ortiz, Rivera, & Greer, 2002) and increasing the rate of learning for textual responding (Tsai & Greer, 2006). However, stimulus-stimulus pairings can often be time consuming and, even then, are not always effective in conditioning new stimuli as reinforcers. The use of peers has often been employed in conditioning new reinforcers for getting children to try new foods (Greer, McCorkle, & Sales, 1998; Greer & Sales, 1997) and inducing swallowing (Greer, Dorow, Williams, McCorkle, & Asnes, 1991). Greer and Singer-Dudek (2008) reported an observational procedure that was effective in converting small plastic discs for five participants or pieces of string for one participant from non-reinforcers to reinforcers for both learning and performance tasks. The present study was conducted in order to determine whether the results of this observational procedure (Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008) would replicate when the neutral stimuli used were strings, since there was only one participant in the Greer and Singer-Dudek study for whom strings were used. Could small pieces of string, which hold neither inherent value nor generalized reinforcement application, be conditioned as reinforcers using this procedure with other participants? In other words, would the results replicate? If so, what would that mean for developing stimuli that require no back up reinforcement or satiation potential for educational applications? Specifically, we wanted to further examine the role of the experimenter in the present study. …","PeriodicalId":236920,"journal":{"name":"The Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention","volume":"126 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"28","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"The Effects of an Observational Intervention on the Acquisition of Reinforcing Properties of a Previously Neutral Stimulus.\",\"authors\":\"Jessica Singer-Dudek, R. D. Greer, Jeannine Schmelzkopf\",\"doi\":\"10.1037/H0100410\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Humans can learn in one of two ways; through direct instruction, or through the observation of others (Greer, Singer-Dudek, & Gautreaux, 2006). Observational learning is the process in which an individual learns a new response/environment relation (one that was not previously in his repertoire) as a result of the observation of another individual receiving contact with the contingencies of reinforcement, punishment, and corrections of incorrect responses (Catania, 2007; Gautreaux, 2005). Greer et al. (2006) state that there are three different functions of observational learning, including 1) the emission of previously acquired operants, 2) the acquisition of new operants through observation, and 3) the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers through observation. Although observational effects on learning new operants has a growing literature (Brody, Lahey, & Combs, 1979; Egel, Richman, & Koegel, 1981; Griffen, Wolery, & Schuster, 1992; Goldstein & Mousetis, 1989; Greer, et. al, 2006; McDonald, Dixon, & Leblanc, 1986; Werts, Caldwell, & Wolery, 1996) and the effects of observation on performance has been studied extensively (Bandura, 1986; Bandura, Adams, & Beyer, 1977; Kazdin, 1973; Ollendick, Dailey, & Shapiro, 1983), the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers through observation appears to be a new type of observational learning that has only very recently been identified (Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008; Greer, Singer-Dudek, Delgado, & Oblak, 2007; Greer, Singer-Dudek, Longano, & Zrinzo, in press; O'Rourke, 2006). This new type of learning, identified as the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers by observation, can be defined as the acquisition of reinforcing properties of previously neutral stimuli as a function of observation (Greer et al., 2006; Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008). Conditioned reinforcement, which typically results from the stimulus-stimulus pairings of unconditioned or conditioned reinforcers with initially neutral stimuli that lead to the acquisition of reinforcing properties for the previously neutral stimuli (Catania, 2007; Donahoe & Palmer, 2004; Dinsmoor, 2004; Kelleher & Gollub, 1962) has been identified in the literature as important for increasing students' communities of reinforcers while decreasing stereotypy (Greer, Becker, Saxe, & Mirabella, 1985; Nuzzolo-Gomez, Leonard, Ortiz, Rivera, & Greer, 2002) and increasing the rate of learning for textual responding (Tsai & Greer, 2006). However, stimulus-stimulus pairings can often be time consuming and, even then, are not always effective in conditioning new stimuli as reinforcers. The use of peers has often been employed in conditioning new reinforcers for getting children to try new foods (Greer, McCorkle, & Sales, 1998; Greer & Sales, 1997) and inducing swallowing (Greer, Dorow, Williams, McCorkle, & Asnes, 1991). Greer and Singer-Dudek (2008) reported an observational procedure that was effective in converting small plastic discs for five participants or pieces of string for one participant from non-reinforcers to reinforcers for both learning and performance tasks. The present study was conducted in order to determine whether the results of this observational procedure (Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008) would replicate when the neutral stimuli used were strings, since there was only one participant in the Greer and Singer-Dudek study for whom strings were used. Could small pieces of string, which hold neither inherent value nor generalized reinforcement application, be conditioned as reinforcers using this procedure with other participants? In other words, would the results replicate? If so, what would that mean for developing stimuli that require no back up reinforcement or satiation potential for educational applications? Specifically, we wanted to further examine the role of the experimenter in the present study. …\",\"PeriodicalId\":236920,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"The Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention\",\"volume\":\"126 1\",\"pages\":\"0\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"1900-01-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"28\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"The Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100410\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100410","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 28

摘要

人类可以通过两种方式学习;通过直接指导,或通过观察他人(Greer, Singer-Dudek, & Gautreaux, 2006)。观察性学习是个体通过观察另一个个体接触到强化、惩罚和纠正错误反应的偶然性,从而学习新的反应/环境关系(以前不存在于个体的技能库中)的过程(卡塔尼亚,2007;Gautreaux, 2005)。Greer等人(2006)指出,观察学习有三种不同的功能,包括1)释放先前习得的操作,2)通过观察习得新的操作,以及3)通过观察习得条件强化。虽然观察效应对新操作的学习有越来越多的文献(Brody, Lahey, & Combs, 1979;Egel, Richman, & Koegel, 1981;Griffen, Wolery, & Schuster, 1992;Goldstein & Mousetis, 1989;Greer等人,2006;McDonald, Dixon, & Leblanc, 1986;Werts, Caldwell, & Wolery, 1996),观察对绩效的影响也得到了广泛的研究(Bandura, 1986;Bandura, Adams, & Beyer, 1977;卡兹丁,1973;Ollendick, Dailey, & Shapiro, 1983),通过观察习得条件强化物似乎是一种最近才被发现的新型观察学习(Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008;格里尔,辛格-杜德克,德尔加多和奥布拉克,2007;格里尔、辛格-杜德克、朗加诺和兹林佐,出版中;O’rourke, 2006)。这种新类型的学习被定义为通过观察获得条件强化物,可以定义为通过观察获得先前中性刺激的强化特性(Greer等人,2006;Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008)。条件强化,通常由非条件或条件强化与最初的中性刺激的刺激-刺激配对产生,导致先前的中性刺激获得强化特性(卡塔尼亚,2007;Donahoe & Palmer, 2004;Dinsmoor, 2004;Kelleher & Gollub, 1962)在文献中被认为是在减少刻板印象的同时增加强化者群体的重要因素(Greer, Becker, Saxe, & Mirabella, 1985;Nuzzolo-Gomez, Leonard, Ortiz, Rivera, & Greer, 2002)和提高文本响应的学习速度(Tsai & Greer, 2006)。然而,刺激-刺激配对往往是耗时的,即使这样,也并不总是有效地将新刺激作为强化物。同伴的使用经常被用来调节新的强化物,让孩子们尝试新的食物(Greer, McCorkle, & Sales, 1998;Greer & Sales, 1997)和诱导吞咽(Greer, Dorow, Williams, McCorkle, & Asnes, 1991)。Greer和Singer-Dudek(2008)报告了一个观察过程,该过程有效地将五个参与者的小塑料盘或一个参与者的绳子从非强化物转化为学习和表现任务的强化物。本研究是为了确定当使用的中性刺激是字符串时,这一观察过程的结果(Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008)是否会重复,因为在Greer和Singer-Dudek研究中只有一个参与者使用了字符串。既不具有固有价值也不具有广义强化应用的小块字符串是否可以与其他参与者一起使用此程序作为强化物?换句话说,结果会重复吗?如果是这样的话,这对于开发不需要备份强化或满足潜力的教育应用刺激意味着什么?具体来说,我们想进一步研究实验者在本研究中的作用。…
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
The Effects of an Observational Intervention on the Acquisition of Reinforcing Properties of a Previously Neutral Stimulus.
Humans can learn in one of two ways; through direct instruction, or through the observation of others (Greer, Singer-Dudek, & Gautreaux, 2006). Observational learning is the process in which an individual learns a new response/environment relation (one that was not previously in his repertoire) as a result of the observation of another individual receiving contact with the contingencies of reinforcement, punishment, and corrections of incorrect responses (Catania, 2007; Gautreaux, 2005). Greer et al. (2006) state that there are three different functions of observational learning, including 1) the emission of previously acquired operants, 2) the acquisition of new operants through observation, and 3) the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers through observation. Although observational effects on learning new operants has a growing literature (Brody, Lahey, & Combs, 1979; Egel, Richman, & Koegel, 1981; Griffen, Wolery, & Schuster, 1992; Goldstein & Mousetis, 1989; Greer, et. al, 2006; McDonald, Dixon, & Leblanc, 1986; Werts, Caldwell, & Wolery, 1996) and the effects of observation on performance has been studied extensively (Bandura, 1986; Bandura, Adams, & Beyer, 1977; Kazdin, 1973; Ollendick, Dailey, & Shapiro, 1983), the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers through observation appears to be a new type of observational learning that has only very recently been identified (Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008; Greer, Singer-Dudek, Delgado, & Oblak, 2007; Greer, Singer-Dudek, Longano, & Zrinzo, in press; O'Rourke, 2006). This new type of learning, identified as the acquisition of conditioned reinforcers by observation, can be defined as the acquisition of reinforcing properties of previously neutral stimuli as a function of observation (Greer et al., 2006; Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008). Conditioned reinforcement, which typically results from the stimulus-stimulus pairings of unconditioned or conditioned reinforcers with initially neutral stimuli that lead to the acquisition of reinforcing properties for the previously neutral stimuli (Catania, 2007; Donahoe & Palmer, 2004; Dinsmoor, 2004; Kelleher & Gollub, 1962) has been identified in the literature as important for increasing students' communities of reinforcers while decreasing stereotypy (Greer, Becker, Saxe, & Mirabella, 1985; Nuzzolo-Gomez, Leonard, Ortiz, Rivera, & Greer, 2002) and increasing the rate of learning for textual responding (Tsai & Greer, 2006). However, stimulus-stimulus pairings can often be time consuming and, even then, are not always effective in conditioning new stimuli as reinforcers. The use of peers has often been employed in conditioning new reinforcers for getting children to try new foods (Greer, McCorkle, & Sales, 1998; Greer & Sales, 1997) and inducing swallowing (Greer, Dorow, Williams, McCorkle, & Asnes, 1991). Greer and Singer-Dudek (2008) reported an observational procedure that was effective in converting small plastic discs for five participants or pieces of string for one participant from non-reinforcers to reinforcers for both learning and performance tasks. The present study was conducted in order to determine whether the results of this observational procedure (Greer & Singer-Dudek, 2008) would replicate when the neutral stimuli used were strings, since there was only one participant in the Greer and Singer-Dudek study for whom strings were used. Could small pieces of string, which hold neither inherent value nor generalized reinforcement application, be conditioned as reinforcers using this procedure with other participants? In other words, would the results replicate? If so, what would that mean for developing stimuli that require no back up reinforcement or satiation potential for educational applications? Specifically, we wanted to further examine the role of the experimenter in the present study. …
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信