New directions in reactance research

IF 2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY
Sandra Sittenthaler, E. Jonas, E. Traut-Mattausch, J. Greenberg
{"title":"New directions in reactance research","authors":"Sandra Sittenthaler, E. Jonas, E. Traut-Mattausch, J. Greenberg","doi":"10.1027/2151-2604/A000221","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"According to reactance theory, formulated by Jack Brehm in 1966, the motivational state of psychological reactance is aroused when an individual perceives any of her or his free behaviors she or he can engage in to be threatened (Brehm, 1966). Reactance theory does not hold that people believe that they have freedom in all daily life situations but that they perceive themselves as having specific freedoms. The amount of reactance experienced when a perceived freedom is threatened depends on the intensity of the threat as well as the importance of the restricted freedom (Brehm, 1966; Brehm & Brehm, 1981; Miron & Brehm, 2006; Worchel & Brehm, 1971). Reactance as research topic has attracted attention in both basic and applied research in areas such as health, marketing, politics, and education. Nevertheless, Miron and Brehm pointed out several research gaps in their review paper on reactance research 40 years later. This review article inspired us to develop this special issue in order to highlight new insightful theory and research concerning reactance processes. ‘‘Reactance’’ is often used as an ex post explanation for different phenomena occurring in applied research, such as research in workor health-related contexts. Yet, interesting new theory-guided research has also been testing a priori hypotheses in different domains over the last decade. And there are still remaining open questions in understanding basic theoretical assumptions concerning the classic social psychology theory of reactance. The purpose of this issue is therefore to present recent research and developments in reactance theory that both offer new knowledge and illuminate issues still in need of resolution. This special issue contains one review article, five empirical articles, and a research spotlight. The review article by Steindl, Jonas, Sittenthaler, TrautMattausch, and Greenberg (2015) introduces the special issue with an overview of research that has been conducted since Miron and Brehm’s review article from 2006. The article presents new developments concerning the measurement of reactance, research on cultural differences in the experience of self-experienced and vicarious reactance, and research revealing different processes involved in reactance. Furthermore, the authors encourage future research to gain a better understanding of the state of psychological reactance per se and to also look at the positive motivational side of reactance. In the empirical paper by Bessarabova, Turner, Fink, and Beary Blustein (2015) data showed that guilt had a direct effect on the affective component of reactance, anger, whereas guilt influenced the cognitive component of reactance via awareness that a message was a guilt appeal. This is an interesting approach in extending reactance theory by relating reactance to guilt appeals and examining the process by which reactance translates into action. The next contribution in persuasion research by Shen and Coles (2015) investigates fear-reactance processes. Results showed that a within-individuals perspective instead of the between-individuals perspective might help to increase our understanding of fear-reactance processes. In the third empirical contribution, De Lemus, Bukowski, Spears, and Telga (2015) found out that the motivational state of reactance can also result from perceived threats to one’s group norms and social identities. More precisely, the authors show that examples of stereotypic women arouse reactance responses in progressive women (feminist identifiers) and examples of counterstereotypic women arouse reactance in more traditional women. The article by Ungar, Sieverding, Schweizer, and Stadnitski (2015) addresses the question of whether an intervention designed to increase the consumption of fruits and vegetables could elicit reactance. Results indicated that even a relatively brief intervention (i.e., eating according to a 5 a day or just 1 more goal for one week) led to reactance that persisted on the same level 1 week later, with effects on attitude, intention, and behavior lasting as much as 4 months later. The final empirical paper presents the validation of a state reactance scale, the Salzburger State Reactance Scale","PeriodicalId":47289,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","volume":"11 1","pages":"203-204"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0000,"publicationDate":"2015-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/A000221","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

According to reactance theory, formulated by Jack Brehm in 1966, the motivational state of psychological reactance is aroused when an individual perceives any of her or his free behaviors she or he can engage in to be threatened (Brehm, 1966). Reactance theory does not hold that people believe that they have freedom in all daily life situations but that they perceive themselves as having specific freedoms. The amount of reactance experienced when a perceived freedom is threatened depends on the intensity of the threat as well as the importance of the restricted freedom (Brehm, 1966; Brehm & Brehm, 1981; Miron & Brehm, 2006; Worchel & Brehm, 1971). Reactance as research topic has attracted attention in both basic and applied research in areas such as health, marketing, politics, and education. Nevertheless, Miron and Brehm pointed out several research gaps in their review paper on reactance research 40 years later. This review article inspired us to develop this special issue in order to highlight new insightful theory and research concerning reactance processes. ‘‘Reactance’’ is often used as an ex post explanation for different phenomena occurring in applied research, such as research in workor health-related contexts. Yet, interesting new theory-guided research has also been testing a priori hypotheses in different domains over the last decade. And there are still remaining open questions in understanding basic theoretical assumptions concerning the classic social psychology theory of reactance. The purpose of this issue is therefore to present recent research and developments in reactance theory that both offer new knowledge and illuminate issues still in need of resolution. This special issue contains one review article, five empirical articles, and a research spotlight. The review article by Steindl, Jonas, Sittenthaler, TrautMattausch, and Greenberg (2015) introduces the special issue with an overview of research that has been conducted since Miron and Brehm’s review article from 2006. The article presents new developments concerning the measurement of reactance, research on cultural differences in the experience of self-experienced and vicarious reactance, and research revealing different processes involved in reactance. Furthermore, the authors encourage future research to gain a better understanding of the state of psychological reactance per se and to also look at the positive motivational side of reactance. In the empirical paper by Bessarabova, Turner, Fink, and Beary Blustein (2015) data showed that guilt had a direct effect on the affective component of reactance, anger, whereas guilt influenced the cognitive component of reactance via awareness that a message was a guilt appeal. This is an interesting approach in extending reactance theory by relating reactance to guilt appeals and examining the process by which reactance translates into action. The next contribution in persuasion research by Shen and Coles (2015) investigates fear-reactance processes. Results showed that a within-individuals perspective instead of the between-individuals perspective might help to increase our understanding of fear-reactance processes. In the third empirical contribution, De Lemus, Bukowski, Spears, and Telga (2015) found out that the motivational state of reactance can also result from perceived threats to one’s group norms and social identities. More precisely, the authors show that examples of stereotypic women arouse reactance responses in progressive women (feminist identifiers) and examples of counterstereotypic women arouse reactance in more traditional women. The article by Ungar, Sieverding, Schweizer, and Stadnitski (2015) addresses the question of whether an intervention designed to increase the consumption of fruits and vegetables could elicit reactance. Results indicated that even a relatively brief intervention (i.e., eating according to a 5 a day or just 1 more goal for one week) led to reactance that persisted on the same level 1 week later, with effects on attitude, intention, and behavior lasting as much as 4 months later. The final empirical paper presents the validation of a state reactance scale, the Salzburger State Reactance Scale
电抗研究的新方向
根据杰克·布雷姆(Jack Brehm) 1966年提出的抗拒理论,当个体感知到她或他可以从事的任何自由行为受到威胁时,心理抗拒的动机状态就会被激发(Brehm, 1966)。抗拒理论并不认为人们相信他们在所有的日常生活情况下都有自由,而是认为他们自己有特定的自由。当感知到的自由受到威胁时,所经历的抗拒程度取决于威胁的强度以及受限制的自由的重要性(Brehm, 1966;Brehm & Brehm, 1981;米隆&布莱姆,2006;Worchel & Brehm, 1971)。电抗作为研究课题,在卫生、市场营销、政治、教育等领域的基础研究和应用研究中都受到了广泛的关注。然而,40年后,Miron和Brehm在他们关于电抗研究的综述论文中指出了一些研究空白。这篇综述文章激发了我们的灵感,为了突出新的有见地的电抗过程理论和研究,我们开发了这个特刊。"电抗"经常被用作应用研究中出现的不同现象的事后解释,例如在工作或与健康有关的背景下进行的研究。然而,在过去十年中,有趣的新理论指导研究也在不同领域测试了先验假设。在理解经典社会心理学抗拒理论的基本理论假设方面,仍然存在一些悬而未决的问题。因此,本期的目的是介绍电抗理论的最新研究和发展,这些研究和发展既提供了新的知识,又阐明了仍然需要解决的问题。本期特刊包含一篇综述文章,五篇实证文章和一个研究焦点。Steindl, Jonas, Sittenthaler, TrautMattausch和Greenberg(2015)的评论文章介绍了这一特殊问题,概述了自2006年Miron和Brehm的评论文章以来进行的研究。本文介绍了电抗的测量、自我体验电抗和替代电抗体验的文化差异研究以及揭示电抗参与的不同过程的研究进展。此外,作者鼓励未来的研究更好地理解心理抗拒本身的状态,并关注抗拒的积极动机方面。在Bessarabova、Turner、Fink和Beary Blustein(2015)的实证论文中,数据显示内疚对抗拒的情感成分愤怒有直接影响,而内疚通过意识到信息是一种内疚的呼吁来影响抗拒的认知成分。这是一种有趣的方法,通过将抗拒与内疚上诉联系起来,并检查抗拒转化为行动的过程,来扩展抗拒理论。Shen和Coles(2015)对说服研究的下一个贡献是研究恐惧-抗拒过程。结果表明,个体内部的视角而不是个体之间的视角可能有助于增加我们对恐惧抗拒过程的理解。在第三个实证贡献中,De Lemus、Bukowski、Spears和Telga(2015)发现,抗拒的动机状态也可能来自对群体规范和社会身份的感知威胁。更准确地说,作者表明,刻板印象中的女性会引起进步女性(女权主义标识符)的抗拒反应,而反刻板印象中的女性会引起更传统女性的抗拒。Ungar, Sieverding, Schweizer和Stadnitski(2015)的文章解决了一个问题,即旨在增加水果和蔬菜消费的干预措施是否会引起抵制。结果表明,即使是相对短暂的干预(例如,每天吃5个或一周多吃一个目标)也会导致抗拒情绪在一周后保持在同一水平,对态度、意图和行为的影响会持续4个月。最后,实证论文提出了一种状态电抗量表——萨尔茨堡状态电抗量表的有效性
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology
Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie-Journal of Psychology PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY-
CiteScore
4.10
自引率
5.60%
发文量
37
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信