T. Carletti, A. Guarino, A. Guazzini, Federica Stefanelli
{"title":"Problem Solving: When Groups Perform Better Than Teammates","authors":"T. Carletti, A. Guarino, A. Guazzini, Federica Stefanelli","doi":"10.18564/jasss.4292","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":": People tend to form groups when they have to solve difficult problems because groups seem to have betterproblem-solvingcapabilitiesthanindividuals. Indeed, duringtheirevolution, humanbeingslearnedthat cooperation is frequently an optimal strategy to solve hard problems both quickly and accurately. The ability of a group to determine a solution to a given problem, once group members alone cannot, has been called “Collective Intelligence\". Such emergent property of the group as a whole is the result of a complex interaction between many factors. Here, we propose a simple and analytically solvable model disentangling the direct link between collective intelligence and the average intelligence of group members. We found that there is a non-linear relation between the collective intelligence of a group and the average intelligence quotient of its members depending on task difficulty. We found three regimes as follows: for simple tasks, the level of collective intelligence of a group is a decreasing function of teammates’ intelligence quotient; when tasks have intermediate difficulties, the relation between collective intelligence and intelligence quotient shows a non-monotone behaviour; for complex tasks, the level of collective intelligence of a group monotonically increases withteammates’intelligencequotientwithphasetransitionsemergingwhenvaryingthelatter’slevel. Although simple and abstract, our model paves the way for future experimental explorations of the link between task complexity, individual intelligence and group performance.","PeriodicalId":14675,"journal":{"name":"J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul.","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2020-06-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"5","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul.","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.18564/jasss.4292","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Abstract
: People tend to form groups when they have to solve difficult problems because groups seem to have betterproblem-solvingcapabilitiesthanindividuals. Indeed, duringtheirevolution, humanbeingslearnedthat cooperation is frequently an optimal strategy to solve hard problems both quickly and accurately. The ability of a group to determine a solution to a given problem, once group members alone cannot, has been called “Collective Intelligence". Such emergent property of the group as a whole is the result of a complex interaction between many factors. Here, we propose a simple and analytically solvable model disentangling the direct link between collective intelligence and the average intelligence of group members. We found that there is a non-linear relation between the collective intelligence of a group and the average intelligence quotient of its members depending on task difficulty. We found three regimes as follows: for simple tasks, the level of collective intelligence of a group is a decreasing function of teammates’ intelligence quotient; when tasks have intermediate difficulties, the relation between collective intelligence and intelligence quotient shows a non-monotone behaviour; for complex tasks, the level of collective intelligence of a group monotonically increases withteammates’intelligencequotientwithphasetransitionsemergingwhenvaryingthelatter’slevel. Although simple and abstract, our model paves the way for future experimental explorations of the link between task complexity, individual intelligence and group performance.