{"title":"The Case for a Heuristic Approach to Account for Suboptimal Choice","authors":"T. Zentall","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2019.140006","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"The commentaries appropriately mention boundary conditions for the less is more effect (Beran, this issue; Carvalho et al., this issue) and the caution that choice behavior that seems suboptimal in the laboratory may be optimal in nature (Vasconcelos et al., this issue). Pisklak et al. (this issue) object to my definition of contrast to describe the difference between probability (or magnitude) of reinforcement expected and obtained but they focus on only one kind of contrast, behavioral contrast. Carvalho et al. question how impulsivity can account for the failure to choose optimally in the ephemeral reward task. The justification comes from research on delay discounting (a measure of impulsivity) in which further delaying both the smaller sooner and the larger later reward can shift preference in the direction of optimality. The same occurs with the ephemeral reward task. With regard to the midsession reversal task, Carvalho et al. question our interpretation of the positive effect on accuracy of reducing the probability of reinforcement for correct choice of S2 (the correct stimulus during the second half of the session). They argue that according to our attentional account, reducing the probability of reinforcement for correct choice of S1 (the correct stimulus during the first half of the session) should have a similar effect. However, during that half of the session, choice of S2 would be an anticipatory error, thus not very helpful as a cue. Instead, we suggest that any manipulation that shifts attention from S2 to S1 (e.g., increasing the response requirement to S2) should improve task accuracy and it does. Finally, I suggest that evolved heuristics may account for an animal’s suboptimal choice but that an animal’s flexibility in dealing with a changing environment may be a useful ability to have and may be worth studying.","PeriodicalId":0,"journal":{"name":"","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2019.140006","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
The commentaries appropriately mention boundary conditions for the less is more effect (Beran, this issue; Carvalho et al., this issue) and the caution that choice behavior that seems suboptimal in the laboratory may be optimal in nature (Vasconcelos et al., this issue). Pisklak et al. (this issue) object to my definition of contrast to describe the difference between probability (or magnitude) of reinforcement expected and obtained but they focus on only one kind of contrast, behavioral contrast. Carvalho et al. question how impulsivity can account for the failure to choose optimally in the ephemeral reward task. The justification comes from research on delay discounting (a measure of impulsivity) in which further delaying both the smaller sooner and the larger later reward can shift preference in the direction of optimality. The same occurs with the ephemeral reward task. With regard to the midsession reversal task, Carvalho et al. question our interpretation of the positive effect on accuracy of reducing the probability of reinforcement for correct choice of S2 (the correct stimulus during the second half of the session). They argue that according to our attentional account, reducing the probability of reinforcement for correct choice of S1 (the correct stimulus during the first half of the session) should have a similar effect. However, during that half of the session, choice of S2 would be an anticipatory error, thus not very helpful as a cue. Instead, we suggest that any manipulation that shifts attention from S2 to S1 (e.g., increasing the response requirement to S2) should improve task accuracy and it does. Finally, I suggest that evolved heuristics may account for an animal’s suboptimal choice but that an animal’s flexibility in dealing with a changing environment may be a useful ability to have and may be worth studying.
评论中适当地提到了边界条件,因为越少越有效(Beran,这个问题;Carvalho et al.,这个问题)以及在实验室中看似次优的选择行为在本质上可能是最优的(Vasconcelos et al.,这个问题)。Pisklak等人(本期)反对我对对比的定义,即描述期望和获得的强化概率(或强度)之间的差异,但他们只关注一种对比,即行为对比。Carvalho等人质疑冲动性如何解释短暂奖励任务中无法做出最佳选择的原因。其理由来自于对延迟折扣(一种衡量冲动的方法)的研究,其中进一步延迟较小的早奖励和较大的晚奖励都可以使偏好向最优方向转移。短暂奖励任务也是如此。关于中间反转任务,Carvalho等人质疑我们对减少正确选择S2(会话后半段的正确刺激)的强化概率对准确性的积极影响的解释。他们认为,根据我们的注意力解释,减少正确选择S1(会话前半段的正确刺激)的强化概率应该具有类似的效果。然而,在这一半的会话中,选择S2将是一个预期错误,因此不是很有帮助的线索。相反,我们建议任何将注意力从S2转移到S1的操作(例如,增加对S2的响应要求)都应该提高任务的准确性,并且确实如此。最后,我认为,进化的启发式可能解释了动物的次优选择,但动物应对不断变化的环境的灵活性可能是一种有用的能力,可能值得研究。