Starting Early: Introducing the Issue

4区 法学 Q1 Social Sciences
J. Brooks-Gunn, L. Markman-Pithers, C. Rouse
{"title":"Starting Early: Introducing the Issue","authors":"J. Brooks-Gunn, L. Markman-Pithers, C. Rouse","doi":"10.1353/FOC.2016.0009","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content: Starting Early: Introducing the Issue Jeanne Brooks-Gunn (bio), Lisa Markman-Pithers (bio), and Cecilia Elena Rouse (bio) Across the nation, more and more people want to see children receive quality education before kindergarten. Public opinion polls suggest that 70 percent of adults favor such programs, partly because of the irresistible idea that “starting early,” and ensuring that children arrive in school ready to learn, is the best way to generate happy, healthy, and productive adults.1 The notion of starting early resonates. Head Start, the federally funded prekindergarten program for children from low-income homes, was a cornerstone of President Lyndon Johnson’s War on Poverty. Even then it was believed that students can’t fully benefit from an elementary education if they don’t arrive at kindergarten ready to learn. Presidents with views as disparate as those of George W. Bush and Barack Obama have called for strengthening early childhood education in their budgets and State of the Union addresses. One reason for the strong support of early childhood education is the seemingly compelling evidence that exposing children to educational experiences when they’re young can have profound effects on later educational, social, and adult outcomes. In fact, as Lynn Karoly points out in this issue, estimates based on some older pre-K programs suggest that every dollar invested in prekindergarten pays off $3 to $17 in terms of benefits, both to the adult individual and to society. That suggests prekindergarten is one of the most effective investments that we can make in children. Indeed, James Heckman of the University of Chicago, a Nobel laureate in economics, has argued that investments made in early childhood are more beneficial and also more cost-effective than those made in later childhood and adolescence.2 The idea that prekindergarten can enhance later learning and adult success is based on the premise that if pre-K programs provide enriching activities more intensively and more intentionally than parents can, then those programs have the potential to boost children’s learning and skill acquisition. In brief, quality pre-K experiences can teach [End Page 3] children the skills that make it easier for them to learn new skills in early elementary school: that is, skills beget skills. Differences in literacy and cognitive skills between children in low-income families and their better-off counterparts are already apparent by age three, or perhaps even earlier.3 The pre-K education programs initiated in the 1960s and 1970s were designed to reduce those gaps by providing quality pre-K education to disadvantaged children, who were less likely to be ready for school. Few pre-K programs existed in the low-income neighborhoods where most disadvantaged children lived, and parents with little income and education were therefore less likely to send their children to prekindergarten than were parents with more resources. And when disadvantaged parents were able to find a pre-K program, it was likely to be of relatively low quality.4 Based on these observations, we would expect that children from disadvantaged environments would benefit the most from pre-K education; that high-quality programs would deliver the greatest benefits; and that children who received such education would benefit more than those who remained at home, cared for by parents, family, and friends. Comparisons between different pre-K programs, on the other hand, shouldn’t show such a stark contrast. These assumptions imply that not all programs would show equal benefits in empirical evaluations. Scholars have called this heterogeneity in outcomes. Interpreting the research requires attention to many factors—family background, comparison group composition, and programs’ quality and intensity. Scholars have extensively studied the efficacy of pre-K programs, especially those offered to four-year-olds. Of more than 100 evaluations of pre-K programs, the vast majority used random assignment of children to receive the preschool treatment or not.5 Most of these experimental programs served children from low-resource families, in keeping with the premise that these children were less likely to have the skills needed for kindergarten and were therefore most likely to benefit. Consequently, we know the most about how preschool influences children from disadvantaged backgrounds. And because many of these...","PeriodicalId":51448,"journal":{"name":"Future of Children","volume":"26 1","pages":"19 - 3"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2016-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1353/FOC.2016.0009","citationCount":"12","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Future of Children","FirstCategoryId":"90","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1353/FOC.2016.0009","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"法学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"Social Sciences","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12

Abstract

In lieu of an abstract, here is a brief excerpt of the content: Starting Early: Introducing the Issue Jeanne Brooks-Gunn (bio), Lisa Markman-Pithers (bio), and Cecilia Elena Rouse (bio) Across the nation, more and more people want to see children receive quality education before kindergarten. Public opinion polls suggest that 70 percent of adults favor such programs, partly because of the irresistible idea that “starting early,” and ensuring that children arrive in school ready to learn, is the best way to generate happy, healthy, and productive adults.1 The notion of starting early resonates. Head Start, the federally funded prekindergarten program for children from low-income homes, was a cornerstone of President Lyndon Johnson’s War on Poverty. Even then it was believed that students can’t fully benefit from an elementary education if they don’t arrive at kindergarten ready to learn. Presidents with views as disparate as those of George W. Bush and Barack Obama have called for strengthening early childhood education in their budgets and State of the Union addresses. One reason for the strong support of early childhood education is the seemingly compelling evidence that exposing children to educational experiences when they’re young can have profound effects on later educational, social, and adult outcomes. In fact, as Lynn Karoly points out in this issue, estimates based on some older pre-K programs suggest that every dollar invested in prekindergarten pays off $3 to $17 in terms of benefits, both to the adult individual and to society. That suggests prekindergarten is one of the most effective investments that we can make in children. Indeed, James Heckman of the University of Chicago, a Nobel laureate in economics, has argued that investments made in early childhood are more beneficial and also more cost-effective than those made in later childhood and adolescence.2 The idea that prekindergarten can enhance later learning and adult success is based on the premise that if pre-K programs provide enriching activities more intensively and more intentionally than parents can, then those programs have the potential to boost children’s learning and skill acquisition. In brief, quality pre-K experiences can teach [End Page 3] children the skills that make it easier for them to learn new skills in early elementary school: that is, skills beget skills. Differences in literacy and cognitive skills between children in low-income families and their better-off counterparts are already apparent by age three, or perhaps even earlier.3 The pre-K education programs initiated in the 1960s and 1970s were designed to reduce those gaps by providing quality pre-K education to disadvantaged children, who were less likely to be ready for school. Few pre-K programs existed in the low-income neighborhoods where most disadvantaged children lived, and parents with little income and education were therefore less likely to send their children to prekindergarten than were parents with more resources. And when disadvantaged parents were able to find a pre-K program, it was likely to be of relatively low quality.4 Based on these observations, we would expect that children from disadvantaged environments would benefit the most from pre-K education; that high-quality programs would deliver the greatest benefits; and that children who received such education would benefit more than those who remained at home, cared for by parents, family, and friends. Comparisons between different pre-K programs, on the other hand, shouldn’t show such a stark contrast. These assumptions imply that not all programs would show equal benefits in empirical evaluations. Scholars have called this heterogeneity in outcomes. Interpreting the research requires attention to many factors—family background, comparison group composition, and programs’ quality and intensity. Scholars have extensively studied the efficacy of pre-K programs, especially those offered to four-year-olds. Of more than 100 evaluations of pre-K programs, the vast majority used random assignment of children to receive the preschool treatment or not.5 Most of these experimental programs served children from low-resource families, in keeping with the premise that these children were less likely to have the skills needed for kindergarten and were therefore most likely to benefit. Consequently, we know the most about how preschool influences children from disadvantaged backgrounds. And because many of these...
尽早开始:介绍问题
为了代替摘要,这里有一个简短的内容摘录:尽早开始:介绍问题Jeanne Brooks-Gunn(传记),Lisa Markman-Pithers(传记)和Cecilia Elena Rouse(传记)在全国范围内,越来越多的人希望看到孩子在幼儿园之前接受优质教育。民意调查显示,70%的成年人赞成这样的项目,部分原因是不可抗拒的想法,“早开始”,并确保孩子们到达学校准备学习,是培养快乐,健康和有生产力的成年人的最好方法尽早开始的想法引起了共鸣。由联邦政府资助的针对低收入家庭儿童的学前教育项目“先发计划”(Head Start)是林登·约翰逊(Lyndon Johnson)总统向贫困宣战的基石。即使在那时,人们也认为,如果学生在幼儿园时没有做好学习的准备,他们就不能从基础教育中充分受益。乔治·w·布什(George W. Bush)和巴拉克·奥巴马(Barack Obama)等观点迥异的总统,都在各自的预算和国情咨文演讲中呼吁加强幼儿教育。早期儿童教育得到大力支持的一个原因是,似乎有令人信服的证据表明,让孩子在年轻时接受教育经历,对他们以后的教育、社会和成年后的成就有深远的影响。事实上,正如Lynn Karoly在本期杂志中指出的那样,基于一些较早的学前教育项目的估计表明,投资于学前教育的每一美元,对成人个人和社会都能带来3到17美元的收益。这表明学前教育是我们可以对孩子进行的最有效的投资之一。事实上,诺贝尔经济学奖得主、芝加哥大学的詹姆斯·赫克曼(James Heckman)认为,在儿童早期进行的投资比在儿童后期和青少年时期进行的投资更有益,也更划算学前教育可以提高孩子以后的学习和成人的成功,这种观点是基于这样一个前提:如果学前教育项目提供比父母更密集、更有意的丰富活动,那么这些项目就有可能促进孩子的学习和技能习得。简而言之,高质量的学前教育经验可以教给孩子们一些技能,使他们更容易在小学早期学习新技能:也就是说,技能催生技能。低收入家庭的孩子和富裕家庭的孩子在读写能力和认知能力上的差异在三岁甚至更早的时候就已经很明显了20世纪60年代和70年代开始的学前教育项目旨在通过向不太可能为上学做好准备的弱势儿童提供高质量的学前教育来缩小这些差距。在大多数弱势儿童居住的低收入社区,几乎没有学前教育项目,因此,收入和受教育程度都较低的父母比拥有更多资源的父母更不可能将孩子送到幼儿园。而当贫困的父母能够找到一个学前教育项目时,它的质量可能相对较低基于这些观察,我们预计来自弱势环境的儿童将从学前教育中获益最多;高质量的项目将带来最大的好处;接受这种教育的孩子会比那些留在家里,由父母、家人和朋友照顾的孩子受益更多。另一方面,不同的学前教育项目之间的比较不应该显示出如此鲜明的对比。这些假设意味着,在经验评估中,并非所有项目都能显示出同等的效益。学者们称之为结果的异质性。解读这项研究需要注意许多因素——家庭背景、对照组组成、节目质量和强度。学者们广泛研究了学前教育项目的效果,尤其是那些面向四岁儿童的项目。在100多个学前教育项目的评估中,绝大多数是随机分配孩子接受或不接受学前教育这些实验项目大多是为来自资源匮乏家庭的孩子服务的,前提是这些孩子不太可能具备幼儿园所需的技能,因此最有可能受益。因此,我们最了解学前教育如何影响来自弱势背景的儿童。因为其中很多……
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Future of Children
Future of Children Multiple-
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊介绍: The Future of Children is a collaboration of the Woodrow Wilson School of Public and International Affairs at Princeton University and the Brookings Institution. The mission of The Future of Children is to translate the best social science research about children and youth into information that is useful to policymakers, practitioners, grant-makers, advocates, the media, and students of public policy. The project publishes two journals and policy briefs each year, and provides various short summaries of our work. Topics range widely -- from income policy to family issues to education and health – with children’s policy as the unifying element. The senior editorial team is diverse, representing two institutions and multiple disciplines.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信