Speed Reading

IF 5.1 Q1 POLYMER SCIENCE
D. Balota
{"title":"Speed Reading","authors":"D. Balota","doi":"10.1177/1529100615623268","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Compared to listening to a speaker, reading seems more effortful. Why? An obvious difference is that the structure of visual language forces the reader to acquire information in a parasitic manner, looking at patterns of straight and squiggly lines, making eye movements to recognize words, and mapping these onto more “natural” auditory language abilities. But are there ways of bypassing (or minimizing) the extra demands of processing visual language? Many would lead us to believe that the answer is yes. Consider the potential benefits of reading 5 times more quickly than you currently read, with no loss in comprehension. For the past five decades (beginning with Evelyn Wood’s speed-reading programs), there have been training programs that claim to dramatically increase the speed of reading, taking advantage of the massive power of the human brain to leave readers unencumbered by the laborious additional demands of converting print to the system used for spoken language. The target article in this issue has brought together a dream team of researchers who have studied reading and rapid processing of visual information to evaluate the potential efficacy of speed-reading programs. Rayner, Schotter, Masson, Potter, and Treiman argue that if one wants to evaluate such programs, it is critical to first understand the processes that are involved in reading. Thus, the authors provide an in-depth review of the extensive literature on reading, from basic aspects of writing systems to higher-level comprehension. They also review the available empirical evidence evaluating the efficacy of speedreading programs. The review is an outstanding resource for anyone who is interested in reading and speed reading. Rayner et al argue from this literature that speed-reading training programs are unlikely to pay off as advertised because of well-established empirical facts about reading. For example, one approach espoused by speed-reading advocates is to fixate on only a few words within each line of text, thereby decreasing the number of time-consuming fixations that most words receive during normal reading. The notion here is that readers can pick up considerable information in the periphery while fixating a particular word or phrase. Rayner et al. point out that visual information quickly degrades in quality as it extends beyond the fovea and parafovea. Thus, by decreasing fixations, a reader will process fewer words, thereby decreasing comprehension. Another more recent approach is to take advantage of smart devices that use apps to present text one word after another at fixation. This strategy eliminates altogether the “wasted time” devoted to eye movements. There is a rich body of work on presenting text sequentially in this manner (called rapid serial visual presentation), and there is indeed evidence that participants can pick up some information at extraordinarily fast presentation rates. The appeal of this approach is captivating (it can be easily experienced via a simple online demonstration advocating one such training program: http:// spritzinc.com/). However, Rayner et al. argue that this procedure also has limitations because there is evidence of a breakdown in the quality of comprehension at fast presentation rates (see Potter, Kroll, & Harris, 1980). Although the procedure is intuitively appealing, it is likely that people using it are poor at evaluating how much information they are actually processing. The quality of comprehension and our ability to know how much we are comprehending (metacomprehension) is a critical issue in evaluating the appeal of speed-reading programs. As an example of our modest metacognitive abilities, consider the following passage that circulated widely on the Internet: “Aoccdrnig to a rscheearch at Cmabrigde Uinervtisy, it deosn't mttaer in waht oredr the ltteers in a wrod are, the olny iprmoetnt tihng is taht the frist and lsat ltteer be at the rghit pclae.” This demo went viral, suggesting that one can rely heavily on context in reading and avoid detailed processing of letter information. However, the perceived ease of reading 623268 PSIXXX10.1177/1529100615623268BalotaSpeed Reading research-article2015","PeriodicalId":18,"journal":{"name":"ACS Macro Letters","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":5.1000,"publicationDate":"2016-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1529100615623268","citationCount":"17","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"ACS Macro Letters","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100615623268","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"POLYMER SCIENCE","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 17

Abstract

Compared to listening to a speaker, reading seems more effortful. Why? An obvious difference is that the structure of visual language forces the reader to acquire information in a parasitic manner, looking at patterns of straight and squiggly lines, making eye movements to recognize words, and mapping these onto more “natural” auditory language abilities. But are there ways of bypassing (or minimizing) the extra demands of processing visual language? Many would lead us to believe that the answer is yes. Consider the potential benefits of reading 5 times more quickly than you currently read, with no loss in comprehension. For the past five decades (beginning with Evelyn Wood’s speed-reading programs), there have been training programs that claim to dramatically increase the speed of reading, taking advantage of the massive power of the human brain to leave readers unencumbered by the laborious additional demands of converting print to the system used for spoken language. The target article in this issue has brought together a dream team of researchers who have studied reading and rapid processing of visual information to evaluate the potential efficacy of speed-reading programs. Rayner, Schotter, Masson, Potter, and Treiman argue that if one wants to evaluate such programs, it is critical to first understand the processes that are involved in reading. Thus, the authors provide an in-depth review of the extensive literature on reading, from basic aspects of writing systems to higher-level comprehension. They also review the available empirical evidence evaluating the efficacy of speedreading programs. The review is an outstanding resource for anyone who is interested in reading and speed reading. Rayner et al argue from this literature that speed-reading training programs are unlikely to pay off as advertised because of well-established empirical facts about reading. For example, one approach espoused by speed-reading advocates is to fixate on only a few words within each line of text, thereby decreasing the number of time-consuming fixations that most words receive during normal reading. The notion here is that readers can pick up considerable information in the periphery while fixating a particular word or phrase. Rayner et al. point out that visual information quickly degrades in quality as it extends beyond the fovea and parafovea. Thus, by decreasing fixations, a reader will process fewer words, thereby decreasing comprehension. Another more recent approach is to take advantage of smart devices that use apps to present text one word after another at fixation. This strategy eliminates altogether the “wasted time” devoted to eye movements. There is a rich body of work on presenting text sequentially in this manner (called rapid serial visual presentation), and there is indeed evidence that participants can pick up some information at extraordinarily fast presentation rates. The appeal of this approach is captivating (it can be easily experienced via a simple online demonstration advocating one such training program: http:// spritzinc.com/). However, Rayner et al. argue that this procedure also has limitations because there is evidence of a breakdown in the quality of comprehension at fast presentation rates (see Potter, Kroll, & Harris, 1980). Although the procedure is intuitively appealing, it is likely that people using it are poor at evaluating how much information they are actually processing. The quality of comprehension and our ability to know how much we are comprehending (metacomprehension) is a critical issue in evaluating the appeal of speed-reading programs. As an example of our modest metacognitive abilities, consider the following passage that circulated widely on the Internet: “Aoccdrnig to a rscheearch at Cmabrigde Uinervtisy, it deosn't mttaer in waht oredr the ltteers in a wrod are, the olny iprmoetnt tihng is taht the frist and lsat ltteer be at the rghit pclae.” This demo went viral, suggesting that one can rely heavily on context in reading and avoid detailed processing of letter information. However, the perceived ease of reading 623268 PSIXXX10.1177/1529100615623268BalotaSpeed Reading research-article2015
快速阅读
与听演讲相比,阅读似乎更费力。为什么?一个明显的区别是,视觉语言的结构迫使读者以寄生的方式获取信息,通过观察直线和弯弯曲曲的线条,通过眼球运动来识别单词,并将这些信息映射到更“自然”的听觉语言能力上。但是有没有办法绕过(或最小化)处理视觉语言的额外需求呢?许多人会让我们相信答案是肯定的。考虑一下阅读速度比现在快5倍的潜在好处,同时不损失理解能力。在过去的五十年里(从伊夫林·伍德的快速阅读项目开始),有一些培训项目声称可以显著提高阅读速度,利用人类大脑的巨大力量,让读者不受将印刷品转换为口语系统的费力的额外要求的阻碍。这期的目标文章汇集了一个研究阅读和视觉信息快速处理的研究人员梦之队,以评估快速阅读程序的潜在功效。Rayner、Schotter、Masson、Potter和Treiman认为,如果想要评估这些程序,首先了解阅读过程是至关重要的。因此,作者对大量关于阅读的文献进行了深入的回顾,从写作系统的基本方面到更高层次的理解。他们还回顾了评估快速阅读项目有效性的现有经验证据。对于任何对阅读和快速阅读感兴趣的人来说,这篇评论都是一个出色的资源。Rayner等人从这些文献中认为,快速阅读训练项目不太可能像宣传的那样有效果,因为关于阅读的经验事实已经得到了证实。例如,快速阅读倡导者支持的一种方法是在每行文本中只关注几个单词,从而减少在正常阅读中大多数单词所接受的耗时的关注次数。这里的概念是,当读者专注于一个特定的单词或短语时,可以在外围获取相当多的信息。Rayner等人指出,当视觉信息延伸到中央窝和副中央窝之外时,其质量会迅速下降。因此,通过减少注视,读者将处理更少的单词,从而降低理解能力。另一种最近的方法是利用智能设备,使用应用程序在固定时一个字接一个字地呈现文本。这个策略完全消除了花在眼球运动上的“浪费时间”。有大量的工作是关于以这种方式依次呈现文本(称为快速连续视觉呈现),确实有证据表明参与者可以以非常快的呈现速度获取一些信息。这种方法的吸引力是令人着迷的(通过一个简单的在线演示,可以很容易地体验到这种培训计划:http:// spritzinc.com/).)然而,Rayner等人认为,这种方法也有局限性,因为有证据表明,在快速呈现速率下,理解质量会下降(见Potter, Kroll, & Harris, 1980)。虽然这个过程在直觉上很吸引人,但使用它的人很可能不善于评估他们实际处理了多少信息。在评估快速阅读项目的吸引力时,理解的质量和我们了解自己理解程度的能力(元理解)是一个关键问题。举个例子来说明我们适度的元认知能力,看看下面这段在互联网上广为流传的文章:“根据剑桥大学的一项研究,单词中字母的顺序如何并不重要,唯一的改进是第一个和最后一个字母的位置正确。”这个演示在网上疯传,表明人们在阅读时可以严重依赖上下文,避免对字母信息的详细处理。然而,感知阅读的轻松度623268 psixxx10 .1177/1529100615623268 balotspeed reading research-article2015
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
10.40
自引率
3.40%
发文量
209
审稿时长
1 months
期刊介绍: ACS Macro Letters publishes research in all areas of contemporary soft matter science in which macromolecules play a key role, including nanotechnology, self-assembly, supramolecular chemistry, biomaterials, energy generation and storage, and renewable/sustainable materials. Submissions to ACS Macro Letters should justify clearly the rapid disclosure of the key elements of the study. The scope of the journal includes high-impact research of broad interest in all areas of polymer science and engineering, including cross-disciplinary research that interfaces with polymer science. With the launch of ACS Macro Letters, all Communications that were formerly published in Macromolecules and Biomacromolecules will be published as Letters in ACS Macro Letters.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信