Examining prejudice towards Middle Eastern persons via a transformation of stimulus functions.

M. Dixon, A. Branon, Becky L. Nastally, N. Mui
{"title":"Examining prejudice towards Middle Eastern persons via a transformation of stimulus functions.","authors":"M. Dixon, A. Branon, Becky L. Nastally, N. Mui","doi":"10.1037/H0100672","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Since September 11th, 2001, the field of psychology has become increasingly interested in the effects terrorism has had on America. For example, there has been a substantial increase in acts of violence (Bar-Tal & Labin, 2001) and prejudicial ideation (Coryn, Beale, & Myers, 2004) toward Muslims and people of Middle Eastern descent. Mainstream psychological accounts for acts of prejudice have included personality orientation (Crowson, DeBacker, & Thoma, 2005), and beliefs of perceived threats to the U.S., social dominance orientation, and self-categorization (Oswald, 2006). While these accounts attempt to isolate predictive factors for prejudicial ideation, they do not focus on observable behavior that is amenable to change. In fact, Sidman has said the \"experimental, theoretical, and applied sciences of Behavior Analysis are untapped resources with respect to the issue of terrorism\" (2003). He goes on to assert that \"terrorism is a behavioral problem,\" for which the solution requires a basic understanding of its maintaining variables. In a traditional behavioral account of terrorism, acts of terror and prejudice related to them would be solely governed by the consequences that follow these classes of behavior. For example, a Middle Eastern man who commits an act of terrorism would be reinforced by social contingencies (praise from fellow terrorists), the sight of a perceived enemy's suffering (images of pain following successful terrorist acts), and possibly by the media 's coverage of the act (instant fame and notoriety). In the same way, an American police officer who calls a person of Middle Eastern descent in for questioning based on his racial profile is also reinforced by social contingencies (praise from a like-minded social circle), the sight of a Middle Eastern detainee in custody, and perhaps a promotion (more money for working hard to keep his neighborhood safe from terrorism). Establishing operations (Michael, 1993) or setting events (Kantor & Smith, 1975) such as being deprived of attention for most of one's life or experiencing economic hardship would only serve to make the above consequences more salient. While a traditional behavioral account explains how direct contingencies can shape prejudicial behavior and acts of terrorism, a more comprehensive account is needed. The traditional account does not address the fact that the targets of prejudice related to terrorism, innocent U.S. and Middle Eastern civilians, have never been directly paired with either the U.S. government or terrorist groups responsible for the 9/11 attacks, respectively. Some behavior analytic researchers have answered the call. In a paper published in 2003, Dixon, Dymond, Rehfeldt, Roche, & Zlomke outlined a more complex account of prejudice and terrorism and set an agenda for empirical studies to follow. Specifically, the article explains how the 9/11 attacks themselves, the resulting emotional responses of rage and hate felt by Americans, and the images of terrorists displayed by the media come to form a frame of coordination (they become equivalent). Through multiple exemplars of these relational frames, as well as frames of opposition and difference, this class is strengthened, resulting in the generalized prejudice Americans have come to experience. The article also offers an analysis of how young Middle Eastern men come to resent the United States and consequently enter a life of terrorism. The above account stems from a Relational Frame Theory (RFT) conceptualization of prejudice and terrorism (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001). RFT suggests that verbally competent humans have the ability to categorize, evaluate, and compare arbitrarily related events, but they learn to justify these relations based on verbally abstracted non-arbitrary features of related events. For example, if two objects are randomly selected (e.g. a ball and a tree) and compared to one another (e.g. \"How is a ball like a tree? …","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"10 1","pages":"295-318"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2009-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"8","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The behavior analyst today","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100672","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8

Abstract

Since September 11th, 2001, the field of psychology has become increasingly interested in the effects terrorism has had on America. For example, there has been a substantial increase in acts of violence (Bar-Tal & Labin, 2001) and prejudicial ideation (Coryn, Beale, & Myers, 2004) toward Muslims and people of Middle Eastern descent. Mainstream psychological accounts for acts of prejudice have included personality orientation (Crowson, DeBacker, & Thoma, 2005), and beliefs of perceived threats to the U.S., social dominance orientation, and self-categorization (Oswald, 2006). While these accounts attempt to isolate predictive factors for prejudicial ideation, they do not focus on observable behavior that is amenable to change. In fact, Sidman has said the "experimental, theoretical, and applied sciences of Behavior Analysis are untapped resources with respect to the issue of terrorism" (2003). He goes on to assert that "terrorism is a behavioral problem," for which the solution requires a basic understanding of its maintaining variables. In a traditional behavioral account of terrorism, acts of terror and prejudice related to them would be solely governed by the consequences that follow these classes of behavior. For example, a Middle Eastern man who commits an act of terrorism would be reinforced by social contingencies (praise from fellow terrorists), the sight of a perceived enemy's suffering (images of pain following successful terrorist acts), and possibly by the media 's coverage of the act (instant fame and notoriety). In the same way, an American police officer who calls a person of Middle Eastern descent in for questioning based on his racial profile is also reinforced by social contingencies (praise from a like-minded social circle), the sight of a Middle Eastern detainee in custody, and perhaps a promotion (more money for working hard to keep his neighborhood safe from terrorism). Establishing operations (Michael, 1993) or setting events (Kantor & Smith, 1975) such as being deprived of attention for most of one's life or experiencing economic hardship would only serve to make the above consequences more salient. While a traditional behavioral account explains how direct contingencies can shape prejudicial behavior and acts of terrorism, a more comprehensive account is needed. The traditional account does not address the fact that the targets of prejudice related to terrorism, innocent U.S. and Middle Eastern civilians, have never been directly paired with either the U.S. government or terrorist groups responsible for the 9/11 attacks, respectively. Some behavior analytic researchers have answered the call. In a paper published in 2003, Dixon, Dymond, Rehfeldt, Roche, & Zlomke outlined a more complex account of prejudice and terrorism and set an agenda for empirical studies to follow. Specifically, the article explains how the 9/11 attacks themselves, the resulting emotional responses of rage and hate felt by Americans, and the images of terrorists displayed by the media come to form a frame of coordination (they become equivalent). Through multiple exemplars of these relational frames, as well as frames of opposition and difference, this class is strengthened, resulting in the generalized prejudice Americans have come to experience. The article also offers an analysis of how young Middle Eastern men come to resent the United States and consequently enter a life of terrorism. The above account stems from a Relational Frame Theory (RFT) conceptualization of prejudice and terrorism (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001). RFT suggests that verbally competent humans have the ability to categorize, evaluate, and compare arbitrarily related events, but they learn to justify these relations based on verbally abstracted non-arbitrary features of related events. For example, if two objects are randomly selected (e.g. a ball and a tree) and compared to one another (e.g. "How is a ball like a tree? …
通过刺激函数的转换考察对中东人的偏见。
自2001年9月11日以来,心理学领域对恐怖主义对美国的影响越来越感兴趣。例如,针对穆斯林和中东后裔的暴力行为(Bar-Tal & Labin, 2001)和偏见(Coryn, Beale, & Myers, 2004)大幅增加。偏见行为的主流心理学解释包括人格取向(Crowson, DeBacker, & Thoma, 2005),以及对美国感知威胁的信念,社会支配取向和自我分类(Oswald, 2006)。虽然这些描述试图隔离偏见的预测因素,但它们并不关注可观察到的可改变的行为。事实上,Sidman曾说过,“行为分析的实验、理论和应用科学是关于恐怖主义问题尚未开发的资源”(2003)。他接着断言,“恐怖主义是一个行为问题”,要解决这个问题,需要对其维持变量有基本的了解。在对恐怖主义的传统行为解释中,恐怖行为和与之相关的偏见将完全由这些行为所带来的后果来决定。例如,一个中东人实施了恐怖主义行为,社会偶发事件(来自恐怖分子同伴的赞扬)、看到敌人所遭受的痛苦(恐怖主义行为成功后的痛苦画面),以及媒体对该行为的报道(瞬间的名声和恶名),都会强化他的行为。同样,一名美国警察因为种族特征而传唤一名中东裔的人问话,也会因为社会偶发事件(来自志同道合的社交圈的赞扬)、看到一名中东裔被拘留者被拘留,或许还会得到晋升(努力保护他的社区免受恐怖主义侵害而获得更多的钱)而得到强化。建立业务(Michael, 1993)或设置事件(Kantor & Smith, 1975),例如在一生的大部分时间里被剥夺注意力或经历经济困难,只会使上述后果更加突出。虽然传统的行为解释解释了直接的偶然事件如何塑造偏见行为和恐怖主义行为,但需要一个更全面的解释。传统的解释没有解决这样一个事实,即与恐怖主义有关的偏见的目标,无辜的美国和中东平民,从来没有直接与美国政府或恐怖组织分别负责9/11袭击。一些行为分析研究人员响应了这一号召。在2003年发表的一篇论文中,Dixon、Dymond、Rehfeldt、Roche和Zlomke对偏见和恐怖主义进行了更复杂的描述,并为后续的实证研究设定了议程。具体来说,这篇文章解释了9/11袭击本身,由此产生的美国人的愤怒和仇恨的情绪反应,以及媒体展示的恐怖分子的形象如何形成一个协调的框架(它们变得等同)。通过这些关系框架的多个例子,以及对立和差异框架,这一阶级得到了加强,从而产生了美国人所经历的普遍偏见。这篇文章还分析了中东的年轻人是如何怨恨美国,并因此走上了恐怖主义的道路。上述解释源于关系框架理论(RFT)对偏见和恐怖主义的概念化(Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001)。RFT表明,语言能力强的人有能力对任意相关的事件进行分类、评估和比较,但他们学习基于相关事件的口头抽象的非任意特征来证明这些关系。例如,如果两个对象是随机选择的(例如一个球和一棵树),并相互比较(例如:“球怎么像树?”...
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信