{"title":"Enhancing students’ fraction magnitude knowledge: A study with students in early elementary education","authors":"Arthur B. Powell","doi":"10.1016/j.jmathb.2023.101042","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The idea of magnitude is central to understanding fractional numbers. To investigate this relationship, we implemented a design research project in an urban school in the northeast of the US to examine the potential of a measuring perspective and the mathematical notion of fraction-<em>of</em>-quantity to enhance second-grade students’ conceptual understanding of fraction magnitude. We used ideas from the history of mathematics and mathematics education within a cultural-historical framework to define fractions and construct tasks. The research team consisted of a university professor, two doctoral students, one of whom was an administrator of the municipal board of education, eight elementary school teachers, and a parent. The research sessions involved 35 students divided into two classes, meeting one hour per session twice a week for 12 weeks or 24 hours. The students manipulated non-symbolic or non-numeric manipulatives (Cuisenaire rods) and learned to talk about specific relations they perceived among them. Through physical manipulations and discourse, students developed the idea that a fraction reports a multiplicative comparison between two commensurable quantities of the same kind. Our results indicate that second-grade students appropriated the concept of the magnitude of fractions-<em>of</em>-quantity and, based on mental manipulations of evoked non-numeric images, constructed symbolic expressions involving fractional comparisons.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47481,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Mathematical Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0000,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"3","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Mathematical Behavior","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0732312323000123","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Abstract
The idea of magnitude is central to understanding fractional numbers. To investigate this relationship, we implemented a design research project in an urban school in the northeast of the US to examine the potential of a measuring perspective and the mathematical notion of fraction-of-quantity to enhance second-grade students’ conceptual understanding of fraction magnitude. We used ideas from the history of mathematics and mathematics education within a cultural-historical framework to define fractions and construct tasks. The research team consisted of a university professor, two doctoral students, one of whom was an administrator of the municipal board of education, eight elementary school teachers, and a parent. The research sessions involved 35 students divided into two classes, meeting one hour per session twice a week for 12 weeks or 24 hours. The students manipulated non-symbolic or non-numeric manipulatives (Cuisenaire rods) and learned to talk about specific relations they perceived among them. Through physical manipulations and discourse, students developed the idea that a fraction reports a multiplicative comparison between two commensurable quantities of the same kind. Our results indicate that second-grade students appropriated the concept of the magnitude of fractions-of-quantity and, based on mental manipulations of evoked non-numeric images, constructed symbolic expressions involving fractional comparisons.
期刊介绍:
The Journal of Mathematical Behavior solicits original research on the learning and teaching of mathematics. We are interested especially in basic research, research that aims to clarify, in detail and depth, how mathematical ideas develop in learners. Over three decades, our experience confirms a founding premise of this journal: that mathematical thinking, hence mathematics learning as a social enterprise, is special. It is special because mathematics is special, both logically and psychologically. Logically, through the way that mathematical ideas and methods have been built, refined and organized for centuries across a range of cultures; and psychologically, through the variety of ways people today, in many walks of life, make sense of mathematics, develop it, make it their own.