As If Time Really Mattered: Temporal Strategies for Neural Coding of Sensory Information

Origins Pub Date : 2018-10-24 DOI:10.4324/9781315789347-16
Eaton Peabody
{"title":"As If Time Really Mattered: Temporal Strategies for Neural Coding of Sensory Information","authors":"Eaton Peabody","doi":"10.4324/9781315789347-16","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Potential strategies for temporal neural processing in the brain and their implications for the design of artificial neural networks are considered. Current connectionist thinking holds that neurons send signals to each other by changes in their average rate of discharge. This implies that there is one output signal per neuron at any given time (scalar coding), and that all neuronal specificity is achieved solely by patterns of synaptic connections. However, information can be carried by temporal codes, in temporal patterns of neural discharges and by relative times of arrival of individual spikes. Temporal coding permits multiplexing of information in the time domain, which potentially increases the flexibility of neural networks. A broadcast model of information transmission is contrasted with the current notion of highly specific connectivity. Evidence for temporal coding in somatoception, audition, electroception, gustation, olfaction and vision is reviewed, and possible neural architectures for temporal information processing are discussed. 1. The role of timing in the brain The human brain is by far the most capable, the most versatile, and the most complex informationprocessing system known to science. For those concerned with problems of artificial intelligence there has long been the dream that once its functional principles are well understood, the design and construction of adaptive devices more powerful than any yet seen could follow in a straightforward manner. Despite great advances, the neurosciences are still far from understanding the nature of the \"neural code\" underlying the detailed workings of the brain. i.e. exactly which information-processing operations are involved. If we choose to view the brain in informational terms, as an adaptive signalling system embedded within an external environment, then the issue of which aspects of neural activity constitute the \"signals\" in the system is absolutely critical to understanding its functioning. It is a question which must be answered before all others, because all functional assumptions, interpretations, and models depend upon the appropriate choice of what processes neurons use to convey information. The role of the time patterns of neural discharges in the transmission and processing of information in the nervous system has been debated since the pulsatile nature of nervous transmission was recognized less than a century ago. Because external stimuli can be physically well-characterized and controlled, the encoding of sensory information has always played a pivotal role in more general conceptions of neural coding. 2. Coding by average discharge rate With the advent of single cell recording techniques in neurophysiology, it was generally assumed that neural information is encoded solely in the average neural discharge rates of neurons (Adrian 1928). This notion of a average discharge rate code, sometimes called the Frequency Coding principle1, has persisted and forms the basis for virtually all neural net design (Feldman 1990) and almost all neuroscientific investigations concerned with information processing (Barlow 1972). While there is much accumulated experimental evidence to support such a principle in many systems, it does not necessarily follow that only average rate codes are used in the nervous coding. From the advent of modern electrophysiology, there were always other conceptions of how sense information could be transmitted (Troland 1921; Troland 1929; Wever & Bray 1937; Boring 1942; Wever 1949). Many other types of codes produce signals which co-vary with average rates, and these other coding schemes may actually contain much higher quality information than average discharge rates. In the auditory nerve, for example, stimulus periodicities below a few kHz are much more precisely represented by interspike interval statistics than by discharge rates (Goldstein & Srulovicz 1977), but because both interval patterns and discharge rate patterns are observed together, it is difficult to determine directly which kinds of codes are functionally operant. However, since rate-coding has become the default assumption of practicing neuroscientists, the burden of proof generally falls on the alternatives. The principle of rate coding has a number of wide-ranging ramifications in the way that neural networks, both wet and dry are conceptualized. A mean rate code entails some time window over which spikes are counted, and depending upon the system, this window is usually thought to be on the order of tens to hundreds of milliseconds or more. Long integration windows can present problems in sensory systems where coherent, detailed percepts can be generated with short 1\"Frequency\" has two meanings, one associated with a rate of events, the other associated with a particular periodicity of events. Frequency Coding implies the former meaning. stimulus durations (e.g. tachistoscopically presented images, tone bursts). The meaningful use of an average discharge rate is also stretched when only a handful of spikes are discharged within an integration window, as often occurs in cortical neurons. Rate coding goes hand in hand with the doctrine of \"specific nerve energies,\" as it was laid out by Müller and Helmholtz (see discussion in (Boring 1933; Boring 1942)). The principle asserts that specific sensory modalities have specific types of sense receptors. Consequently it is by virtue of connection to a given type of receptor that a given neuron is interpreted to be sending a signal related to a particular quality (a visual signal as opposed to a smell). Helmholtz through his study of the cochlea elevated this principle to also include quality differences within a sense modality. Thus, in Helmholtz's view, because particular auditory nerve fibers are connected to receptors at specific places on the cochlear partition, and hence have different frequency sensitivities, they signal different pure tone pitches by virtue of their connectivity. Coding exclusively by average discharge rate necessitates this kind of \"labelled line\" or \"place\" coding because there is no other means internal to the spike train itself for conveying what kind of signal it is (e.g. a taste vs. a sound; the semantics of the message). While the doctrine of specific nerve energies does not mandate that average rate be the signal encoded in the spike train (e.g. see the discussion of Troland's resonance-frequency theory of hearing (Boring 1942)), it has generally been taken on faith that sensory coding could be accomplished solely by rate-place codes. Unless temporal patterns are immediately obvious and impossible to ignore, looking elsewhere into coding alternatives has generally been regarded by neuroscientists as wasted effort. In tandem with exclusive use of rate codes, it has often been assumed that there is no usable temporal structure in spike trains, i.e. spike trains can be functionally described as a Poisson process with one independent parameter, the mean rate of arrivals. As a result, in many higherlevel models of neuronal networks, the temporal dynamics of spike generation are ignored in favor of mean rates or discharge probabilities. One far reaching consequence of these high level functional descriptions is that the neural output signal in any given time period is conceived as a scalar quantity. This effectively rules out the multiplexing of signals in the time domain, which would require a finer grained representation of time and a different (e.g. Fourier) interpretation of the signal. Since only one output signal can be sent from each neural element, multiple input signals converging on a given element must be converted into one output signal. An analogy could be made to a telegraph network which recieves messages from a hundred stations, but can only transmit one message to all of its hundred connecting stations. Each additional signal must compete with all others at each node. In contrast, a station which has several frequency bands available can process meaningful information in one or two bands and relay the other messages unchanged. Even the assumption that all postsynaptic neurons receive the same message can be called into question, since conduction blocks in different branches of axon trees can filter the spike trains that arrive at the respective synapses (Bittner 1968; Raymond & Lettvin 1978; Waxman 1978; Raymond 1979; Wasserman 1992). Instead of one informationally-passive output line fanning out to send the same signal to all postsynaptic elements, a branching structure is created which sequentially filters the signals. Thus the shift from scalars to multidimensional signalling and the inclusion of axonal operations can drastically the functional topology of the network, and with it the flexibility of infomation processing. Largely because of the ordering in cortical maps of retinotopic positions, cochleotopic positions, and somatotopic positions, it has long been assumed that the cortex is a spatial pattern processor. This view of cortical structures was crystallized in a set of far-reaching and of provocative papers by David Marr (Marr 1970; McNaughton & Nadel 1990; Marr 1991). In these papers Marr proposed general information processing mechanisms for the major cortical structures in the brain: the cerebral cortex, the hippocampus (\"archicortex\") and the cerebellar cortex. While it seems abundantly clear that spatially ordered maps are functionally very important, there is no inherent reason why the cortex must be only a spatial processor, why it cannot also be structured so as to effect time-space transformations (Pitts & McCulloch 1947). Alternative timeplace architectures, such as those first articulated by Licklider (Licklider 1951) and Braitenberg (Braitenberg 1961; Braitenberg 1967) take advantage of spatial orderings to perform computations in the time domain. After a long period of relative neglect, the recent discoveries of neuronal synchronies in","PeriodicalId":82238,"journal":{"name":"Origins","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2018-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"14","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Origins","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315789347-16","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14

Abstract

Potential strategies for temporal neural processing in the brain and their implications for the design of artificial neural networks are considered. Current connectionist thinking holds that neurons send signals to each other by changes in their average rate of discharge. This implies that there is one output signal per neuron at any given time (scalar coding), and that all neuronal specificity is achieved solely by patterns of synaptic connections. However, information can be carried by temporal codes, in temporal patterns of neural discharges and by relative times of arrival of individual spikes. Temporal coding permits multiplexing of information in the time domain, which potentially increases the flexibility of neural networks. A broadcast model of information transmission is contrasted with the current notion of highly specific connectivity. Evidence for temporal coding in somatoception, audition, electroception, gustation, olfaction and vision is reviewed, and possible neural architectures for temporal information processing are discussed. 1. The role of timing in the brain The human brain is by far the most capable, the most versatile, and the most complex informationprocessing system known to science. For those concerned with problems of artificial intelligence there has long been the dream that once its functional principles are well understood, the design and construction of adaptive devices more powerful than any yet seen could follow in a straightforward manner. Despite great advances, the neurosciences are still far from understanding the nature of the "neural code" underlying the detailed workings of the brain. i.e. exactly which information-processing operations are involved. If we choose to view the brain in informational terms, as an adaptive signalling system embedded within an external environment, then the issue of which aspects of neural activity constitute the "signals" in the system is absolutely critical to understanding its functioning. It is a question which must be answered before all others, because all functional assumptions, interpretations, and models depend upon the appropriate choice of what processes neurons use to convey information. The role of the time patterns of neural discharges in the transmission and processing of information in the nervous system has been debated since the pulsatile nature of nervous transmission was recognized less than a century ago. Because external stimuli can be physically well-characterized and controlled, the encoding of sensory information has always played a pivotal role in more general conceptions of neural coding. 2. Coding by average discharge rate With the advent of single cell recording techniques in neurophysiology, it was generally assumed that neural information is encoded solely in the average neural discharge rates of neurons (Adrian 1928). This notion of a average discharge rate code, sometimes called the Frequency Coding principle1, has persisted and forms the basis for virtually all neural net design (Feldman 1990) and almost all neuroscientific investigations concerned with information processing (Barlow 1972). While there is much accumulated experimental evidence to support such a principle in many systems, it does not necessarily follow that only average rate codes are used in the nervous coding. From the advent of modern electrophysiology, there were always other conceptions of how sense information could be transmitted (Troland 1921; Troland 1929; Wever & Bray 1937; Boring 1942; Wever 1949). Many other types of codes produce signals which co-vary with average rates, and these other coding schemes may actually contain much higher quality information than average discharge rates. In the auditory nerve, for example, stimulus periodicities below a few kHz are much more precisely represented by interspike interval statistics than by discharge rates (Goldstein & Srulovicz 1977), but because both interval patterns and discharge rate patterns are observed together, it is difficult to determine directly which kinds of codes are functionally operant. However, since rate-coding has become the default assumption of practicing neuroscientists, the burden of proof generally falls on the alternatives. The principle of rate coding has a number of wide-ranging ramifications in the way that neural networks, both wet and dry are conceptualized. A mean rate code entails some time window over which spikes are counted, and depending upon the system, this window is usually thought to be on the order of tens to hundreds of milliseconds or more. Long integration windows can present problems in sensory systems where coherent, detailed percepts can be generated with short 1"Frequency" has two meanings, one associated with a rate of events, the other associated with a particular periodicity of events. Frequency Coding implies the former meaning. stimulus durations (e.g. tachistoscopically presented images, tone bursts). The meaningful use of an average discharge rate is also stretched when only a handful of spikes are discharged within an integration window, as often occurs in cortical neurons. Rate coding goes hand in hand with the doctrine of "specific nerve energies," as it was laid out by Müller and Helmholtz (see discussion in (Boring 1933; Boring 1942)). The principle asserts that specific sensory modalities have specific types of sense receptors. Consequently it is by virtue of connection to a given type of receptor that a given neuron is interpreted to be sending a signal related to a particular quality (a visual signal as opposed to a smell). Helmholtz through his study of the cochlea elevated this principle to also include quality differences within a sense modality. Thus, in Helmholtz's view, because particular auditory nerve fibers are connected to receptors at specific places on the cochlear partition, and hence have different frequency sensitivities, they signal different pure tone pitches by virtue of their connectivity. Coding exclusively by average discharge rate necessitates this kind of "labelled line" or "place" coding because there is no other means internal to the spike train itself for conveying what kind of signal it is (e.g. a taste vs. a sound; the semantics of the message). While the doctrine of specific nerve energies does not mandate that average rate be the signal encoded in the spike train (e.g. see the discussion of Troland's resonance-frequency theory of hearing (Boring 1942)), it has generally been taken on faith that sensory coding could be accomplished solely by rate-place codes. Unless temporal patterns are immediately obvious and impossible to ignore, looking elsewhere into coding alternatives has generally been regarded by neuroscientists as wasted effort. In tandem with exclusive use of rate codes, it has often been assumed that there is no usable temporal structure in spike trains, i.e. spike trains can be functionally described as a Poisson process with one independent parameter, the mean rate of arrivals. As a result, in many higherlevel models of neuronal networks, the temporal dynamics of spike generation are ignored in favor of mean rates or discharge probabilities. One far reaching consequence of these high level functional descriptions is that the neural output signal in any given time period is conceived as a scalar quantity. This effectively rules out the multiplexing of signals in the time domain, which would require a finer grained representation of time and a different (e.g. Fourier) interpretation of the signal. Since only one output signal can be sent from each neural element, multiple input signals converging on a given element must be converted into one output signal. An analogy could be made to a telegraph network which recieves messages from a hundred stations, but can only transmit one message to all of its hundred connecting stations. Each additional signal must compete with all others at each node. In contrast, a station which has several frequency bands available can process meaningful information in one or two bands and relay the other messages unchanged. Even the assumption that all postsynaptic neurons receive the same message can be called into question, since conduction blocks in different branches of axon trees can filter the spike trains that arrive at the respective synapses (Bittner 1968; Raymond & Lettvin 1978; Waxman 1978; Raymond 1979; Wasserman 1992). Instead of one informationally-passive output line fanning out to send the same signal to all postsynaptic elements, a branching structure is created which sequentially filters the signals. Thus the shift from scalars to multidimensional signalling and the inclusion of axonal operations can drastically the functional topology of the network, and with it the flexibility of infomation processing. Largely because of the ordering in cortical maps of retinotopic positions, cochleotopic positions, and somatotopic positions, it has long been assumed that the cortex is a spatial pattern processor. This view of cortical structures was crystallized in a set of far-reaching and of provocative papers by David Marr (Marr 1970; McNaughton & Nadel 1990; Marr 1991). In these papers Marr proposed general information processing mechanisms for the major cortical structures in the brain: the cerebral cortex, the hippocampus ("archicortex") and the cerebellar cortex. While it seems abundantly clear that spatially ordered maps are functionally very important, there is no inherent reason why the cortex must be only a spatial processor, why it cannot also be structured so as to effect time-space transformations (Pitts & McCulloch 1947). Alternative timeplace architectures, such as those first articulated by Licklider (Licklider 1951) and Braitenberg (Braitenberg 1961; Braitenberg 1967) take advantage of spatial orderings to perform computations in the time domain. After a long period of relative neglect, the recent discoveries of neuronal synchronies in
好像时间真的很重要:感官信息的神经编码的时间策略
考虑了大脑中时间神经处理的潜在策略及其对人工神经网络设计的影响。当前的连接主义思想认为,神经元通过其平均放电速率的变化向彼此发送信号。这意味着每个神经元在任何给定的时间都有一个输出信号(标量编码),并且所有神经元的特异性都是通过突触连接的模式来实现的。然而,信息可以通过时间编码、神经放电的时间模式和单个峰值到达的相对时间来传递。时间编码允许信息在时域内进行多路复用,这可能会增加神经网络的灵活性。信息传输的广播模型与当前高度特定连接的概念形成对比。本文综述了体感觉、听觉、电感觉、味觉、嗅觉和视觉中时间编码的证据,并讨论了可能的时间信息处理神经结构。1. 时间在大脑中的作用到目前为止,人类的大脑是科学上已知的最能干、最多才多艺、最复杂的信息处理系统。对于那些关心人工智能问题的人来说,长期以来一直有这样一个梦想:一旦人们很好地理解了人工智能的功能原理,那么设计和建造比任何迄今为止看到的更强大的自适应设备就可以以一种直接的方式进行。尽管取得了巨大的进步,但神经科学仍远未理解隐藏在大脑详细运作之下的“神经密码”的本质。即具体涉及哪些信息处理操作。如果我们选择从信息的角度来看待大脑,将其视为嵌入外部环境中的自适应信号系统,那么神经活动的哪些方面构成了系统中的“信号”,这一问题对于理解其功能至关重要。这是一个必须首先回答的问题,因为所有的功能假设、解释和模型都取决于神经元用来传递信息的过程的适当选择。神经放电的时间模式在神经系统中信息的传递和处理中的作用,自不到一个世纪前神经传递的脉动性被认识以来一直存在争议。由于外部刺激可以在物理上很好地表征和控制,感觉信息的编码在更一般的神经编码概念中一直起着关键作用。2. 随着神经生理学中单细胞记录技术的出现,人们普遍认为神经信息只被编码在神经元的平均神经放电率中(Adrian 1928)。这种平均放电率编码的概念,有时被称为频率编码原则,一直存在并形成了几乎所有神经网络设计(Feldman 1990)和几乎所有与信息处理有关的神经科学研究(Barlow 1972)的基础。虽然在许多系统中积累了大量的实验证据来支持这一原则,但这并不一定意味着神经编码中只使用平均速率编码。从现代电生理学的出现开始,就一直有关于如何传递感官信息的其他概念(Troland 1921;托兰1929;Wever & Bray 1937;无聊的1942;威夫1949)。许多其他类型的编码产生与平均放电率共变的信号,这些其他编码方案实际上可能包含比平均放电率高得多的质量信息。例如,在听觉神经中,低于几千赫的刺激周期用脉冲间隔统计比放电率更精确地表示(Goldstein & Srulovicz 1977),但由于间隔模式和放电率模式都是一起观察的,因此很难直接确定哪种编码在功能上是有效的。然而,由于速率编码已经成为神经科学家的默认假设,举证的责任通常落在替代假设上。速率编码原理在神经网络(干湿两种)概念化的方式中有许多广泛的分支。平均速率代码需要一些时间窗口,在这个时间窗口上计算尖峰,根据系统的不同,这个窗口通常被认为是几十到几百毫秒或更多。“频率”有两个含义,一个与事件的频率有关,另一个与事件的特定周期性有关。频率编码暗示了前一种含义。刺激持续时间(例如,视速呈现的图像,音调爆发)。 当在一个整合窗口内只有少数尖峰放电时,平均放电率的有意义使用也会被拉伸,这通常发生在皮质神经元中。速率编码与“特定神经能量”学说密切相关,这是由m<e:1>勒和亥姆霍兹提出的(见《无聊1933》中的讨论;无聊的1942)。该原理断言,特定的感觉模式有特定类型的感觉受体。因此,通过与特定类型的受体的连接,特定神经元被解释为发送与特定质量相关的信号(与气味相反的视觉信号)。Helmholtz通过他对耳蜗的研究,将这一原则提升到也包括感觉模态中的质量差异。因此,在Helmholtz看来,由于特定的听觉神经纤维与耳蜗分区上特定位置的受体相连,因此具有不同的频率灵敏度,它们凭借其连通性发出不同的纯音音高。仅根据平均放电率进行编码就需要这种“标记线”或“位置”编码,因为在尖峰序列本身内部没有其他方法来传达它是什么类型的信号(例如,味道vs声音;消息的语义)。虽然特定神经能量学说并没有强制要求平均频率是编码在脉冲序列中的信号(例如,参见特兰的听觉共振频率理论(Boring 1942)的讨论),但人们普遍认为,感觉编码只能通过频率位置编码来完成。除非时间模式立即变得明显且无法忽视,否则神经科学家通常认为,在其他地方寻找编码替代方案是浪费精力。随着速率码的独家使用,通常假设在尖峰序列中没有可用的时间结构,即尖峰序列可以被功能地描述为具有一个独立参数(平均到达率)的泊松过程。因此,在许多高级神经网络模型中,峰值产生的时间动态被忽略,而倾向于平均速率或放电概率。这些高级功能描述的一个深远影响是,在任何给定时间段内的神经输出信号都被认为是一个标量。这有效地排除了信号在时域中的多路复用,这将需要更细粒度的时间表示和信号的不同(例如傅里叶)解释。由于每个神经单元只能发送一个输出信号,因此收敛于给定单元的多个输入信号必须转换为一个输出信号。可以做一个类比,一个电报网络从100个站点接收信息,但只能向所有的100个连接站点发送一条信息。每个附加信号必须在每个节点上与所有其他信号竞争。相比之下,一个有几个可用频带的电台可以在一个或两个频带中处理有意义的信息,而不加改变地转发其他信息。甚至所有突触后神经元接收相同信息的假设也可能受到质疑,因为轴突树不同分支的传导阻滞可以过滤到达各自突触的尖峰序列(Bittner 1968;Raymond & Lettvin 1978;维克斯曼1978;雷蒙德1979;沃瑟曼1992)。而不是一个信息被动输出线扇形向所有突触后元件发送相同的信号,一个分支结构被创建,按顺序过滤信号。因此,从标量到多维信号的转变以及轴突操作的包含可以极大地改变网络的功能拓扑结构,并随之改变信息处理的灵活性。很大程度上是因为皮层中视网膜异位、耳蜗异位和躯体异位的排列顺序,长期以来人们一直认为皮层是一个空间模式处理器。这种关于大脑皮层结构的观点在David Marr (Marr 1970;McNaughton & Nadel 1990;马尔1991)。在这些论文中,Marr提出了大脑中主要皮层结构的一般信息处理机制:大脑皮层、海马体(“皮质”)和小脑皮层。虽然空间有序的地图在功能上非常重要,这一点似乎非常清楚,但没有内在的理由说明为什么大脑皮层必须只是一个空间处理器,为什么它不能被结构化以实现时空转换(Pitts & McCulloch 1947)。可选择的时间地点架构,例如由Licklider (Licklider 1951)和Braitenberg (Braitenberg 1961;britenberg(1967)利用空间排序来执行时域的计算。 经过长时间的相对忽视,最近发现的神经元同步
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信