Is the lack of appropriate cognitive demand the primary driver of dementia risk?

Q3 Medicine
Thomas R. Wood, Josh Turknett
{"title":"Is the lack of appropriate cognitive demand the primary driver of dementia risk?","authors":"Thomas R. Wood,&nbsp;Josh Turknett","doi":"10.1002/lim2.70","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>With the general trend of increasing lifespan alongside population-level success in reducing the mortality from cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases, the population burden of Alzheimer's disease (AD) has steadily accelerated. In the United States, the mortality rate due to AD has increased from less than 0.5 per 100,000 in 1980 to approximately 30 per 100,000 in 2019.<span><sup>1</sup></span> Worldwide, the number of individuals with dementia is expected to at least triple by 2050,<span><sup>2</sup></span> with some reports suggesting that AD has the potential to bankrupt healthcare systems.<span><sup>3</sup></span> At the same time, the development of, and investment in, multiple pharmacological agents directed toward modifying the pathological “hallmarks” of AD have yielded disappointing results. Existing pharmaceuticals offer modest symptomatic benefits, at best, without modifying the course of the disease. Taken together, these factors highlight the urgent need for a critical reappraisal of the underlying risk factors for AD and potential interventions.</p><p>In an attempt to reframe potential preventative and therapeutic approaches to AD, we recently proposed a model that suggests demand–function coupling in the brain is the critical upstream factor driving long-term cognitive function.<span><sup>4</sup></span> In this model, we describe how the health and function of any tissue, including the brain, is shaped by the demands placed upon it. In the setting of increased demand, demand–function coupling drives increases in growth and function, but also upregulates processes of cellular repair and regeneration. The result is a tissue that is more resilient, plastic, and with a greater capacity for increased work output in the face of increased requirements. In this way, we propose that the structure and function of the brain are driven by the demands placed upon it, much as cardiac and skeletal muscles respond to exercise. And as muscle or cardiovascular function decline with bed rest or detraining, the structure and function of the brain decline in a coupled manner when adequate demands are not placed upon it. Cognitive decline is then essentially an expression of “frailty” of the brain - defined as lacking additional capacity to function above basic requirements - as a result of reduced demand. Although it may appear subtle, this reframing of the cascade may be critical in understanding the disease process and intervening as healthcare providers. Whereas the prevailing explanations to date have described cognitive activity as a mitigating force; in this model, we propose that cognitive demand instead impacts the primary pathogenetic process. In fact, given the established phenomenon of demand coupling in neural tissue, we believe that this model provides the most parsimonious account of disease pathogenesis.</p><p>In order to explain the approach, we first made two suggestions for a cognitive framework that we believe are essential if we are to better understand the mechanistic underpinnings of dementia and attempt to intervene in the disease process. The first suggestion was that we should separate late-onset or “sporadic” AD from early-onset familial AD.<span><sup>4</sup></span> Though they both have historically been labeled as forms of AD due to shared neuropathological hallmarks at autopsy, these are completely different diseases with respect to their risk factors and disease course. As such, our model only discusses late-onset AD, which we call age-related dementia (ARD), which is preceded by a period of age-related cognitive decline (ARCD).</p><p>The second suggestion was that we should take an opportunity to explore ARD etiology and intervention points from a different level of biological organization. Every biological phenomenon can be described and characterized at multiple levels of biological organization, from the level of cells and molecules to that of populations and ecosystems.<span><sup>5</sup></span> Every disease phenomenon therefore has a level of analysis and characterization best suited to a coherent explanation of pathogenesis, which is typically the level most upstream in the causal chain. Characterizations of downstream consequences at lower levels of organization, regardless of their level of detail, do not necessarily confer additional or greater explanatory power. In the case of ARD, the field has generally focused on granular, lower level molecular and neuropathological details. This is despite the fact that a growing body of evidence indicates that lifestyle and environment—representing the organismal or societal level of biological organization—are the ultimate level of causation for ARD. If this is truly the case, then interventions at the organismal level are those that are likely to be most impactful, also explaining the general failure (to date) of interventions targeted at the molecular level of AD and ARD.</p><p>One issue with changing the level of organization at which we intervene in a disease process is the fact that it requires us to divest ourselves from thinking purely in terms of well-defined biochemical mechanisms. When developing pharmaceutical agents, it is critical to target a single mechanism while minimizing off-target effects. By comparison, when intervening at the organismal level an intervention may be able to generate powerful results without ever having complete knowledge of its biochemical effects. Indeed, the lack of one single mechanism at the molecular or cellular level of organization has seemed to be a fundamental problem when trying to institute lifestyle medicine at the societal level within medical systems that are singularly focused on the mechanism of action of a given intervention. A good example of this is physical activity, which is perhaps the most impactful single intervention for the treatment and prevention of noncommunicable disease.<span><sup>6</sup></span> Increases in exercise and non-exercise physical activities are uniformly recommended by all government, non-governmental, and health-related organizations despite the fact we are still not entirely sure how physical activity “works.” A number of mechanisms linking physical activity and health, including both acute and chronic functional adaptations to exercise, have been elucidated and documented over several decades. However, even in recent years, we have continued to discover mechanisms by which exercise modulates whole-body health and function, including novel pathways involving irisin, MOTS-c (mitochondrial open reading frame of the 12S rRNA type-c), and Lac-Phe (<i>N</i>-lactoyl-phenylalanine) as just a few examples published in high-impact journals with a large amount of associated press and media interest.<span><sup>7-9</sup></span> If we accept that organismal-level interventions with pleiotropic (and sometimes not fully understood) effects may have the greatest benefit on certain aspects of human health, this allows us to reframe our approach to diseases where few inroads have been made so far, such as ARD.</p><p>When discussing the demand model of ARD with clinicians and researchers with expertise in dementia and neurogenerative conditions, it has not been uncommon for them to ask about other established risk factors, particularly those related to general health and the environment. We must therefore make it very clear that we believe that the environmental component of ARCD and ARD is well-established,<span><sup>10-14</sup></span> and should be an important part of any model for disease etiology or intervention. This includes evidence for significantly increased risk of dementia as well as worse cognitive function associated with reduced physical activity,<span><sup>10, 15-17</sup></span> poor sleep,<span><sup>10, 18, 19</sup></span> metabolic disease,<span><sup>20, 21</sup></span> nutrient deficiencies,<span><sup>14, 22</sup></span> hormonal insufficiency,<span><sup>23</sup></span> altered body composition,<span><sup>24-27</sup></span> and social isolation or poor social support.<span><sup>28</sup></span> Early evidence is also mounting to suggest that lifestyle and environmental modification has the potential to prevent or even reverse ARD, especially when initiated early in the disease process.<span><sup>14, 29, 30</sup></span> A prime example of this is the pioneering work by Prof. David Smith and colleagues, who showed that intervention with B vitamins to lower elevated homocysteine slowed the rate of brain atrophy and cognitive decline, particularly in the setting of adequate omega-3 fatty acid status.<span><sup>22</sup></span> Importantly, the environmental influences on ARD risk map directly to the six evidence-based pillars of lifestyle medicine (Figure 1). We therefore caution against thinking of factors as mutually exclusive, and instead believe the above lifestyle end environmental factors to be critical components required for optimal demand–function coupling in the brain. In order to adapt to increased demand in a tissue, that tissue must also have adequate nutrient availability, hormonal and trophic support, and vascular supply, as well as the absence of toxic exposures and opportunity for sleep recovery and adaptation (e.g., sleep). Insufficiencies in any one of these factors would likely undermine the health-promoting adaptations to cognitive demand. To return to the comparison to exercise, an individual may have all the components required to build healthy muscle tissue—nutrition, hormonal status, sleep, etc.—but in the absence of physical exercise they will not improve their fitness. We believe that all the evidence points to brain function, unsurprisingly, being the same.</p><p>To move beyond the hypothetical, what is the evidence for the primary importance of cognitive demand and what might that mean for the future of AD research and intervention? A recent meta-analysis by Yu et al.<span><sup>14</sup></span> found that cognitive demand late in life was the single most protective factor against AD. Similarly, multiple epidemiological studies suggest that the loss of work-related cognitive stimulus due to retirement results in a parallel increase in the risk of cognitive decline, even after adjusting for confounders that might explain both early retirement and cognitive decline such as comorbid medical conditions.<span><sup>31-33</sup></span> The negative effect of loss of cognitive stimulus is also reversible, as evidenced by increased risk of cognitive decline in those that lose sensory function (e.g., sight, smell, or hearing), which can be overcome by interventions that restore the impaired sense.<span><sup>34-38</sup></span> Other types of beneficial cognitive demand including bilingualism or playing a musical instrument, with greater benefits on brain structure in those for whom the practice is more challenging.<span><sup>39-45</sup></span> Coordinative exercise (e.g., yoga, tai chi, or dancing) also appears to have greater benefits on brain structure and function compared to intensity-matched exercise without a coordination component.<span><sup>46, 47</sup></span> Even structured brain training can have carryover to real-world improvements in memory and executive functions.<span><sup>39, 48-52</sup></span> Although they may initially appear disparate, the upshot of these strands of evidence is that an individual may be able to pick any skill or challenge they are particularly interested in as a way to increase cognitive demands. This would also increase both enjoyment and compliance, as well as making it more likely that these skills may be applied in a social or group setting, which would expectedly provide additional benefits.<span><sup>53</sup></span></p><p>We must acknowledge that limitations exist with the current model and there remains much work to be done in order to determine whether it might help shape the future of interventions for ARD. Cognitive demand is difficult to quantify, and therefore the model is difficult to test. From an implementation standpoint, one might hope that better public awareness of the importance of cognitive demand could drive behavior change and increased cognitive demand at the individual level; however, similar approaches to physical activity have yet to be broadly successful and personalized interventions are likely to be needed.<span><sup>54</sup></span> It is also not yet known whether cognitive demand is both necessary and sufficient to explain ARCD and ARD, and any current attempt to prevent or reverse cognitive decline in a holistic manner should include multi-modal interventions such as those employed in the FINGER (Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability) trial.<span><sup>55</sup></span> This hampers the ability to dissect the relevant contributions of different lifestyle factors, including cognitive demand, on the risk of ARCD and ARD. However, novel trial methodologies could be employed to sequentially alter individual risk factors to determine the relative contributions of cognitive demand.<span><sup>4, 56, 57</sup></span> Additional work could also be done in preclinical experiments such as in rodent models. For instance, we could examine whether the brain is still able to beneficially adapt to increased cognitive demands in the setting of suboptimal nutrient status, general health, or with the presence of toxic exposures such as particulate atmospheric pollution. As an example, work in a mouse model of elevated homocysteine—an important risk factor for AD in humans as mentioned above—found that increased cognitive demand through environmental enrichment was still able to result in beneficial trophic responses in the brain.<span><sup>58</sup></span> If this was the case in a broad range of models of the environmental risk factors for AD in humans, then that would support the proposed model.</p><p>Although there is clearly much to be done, we hope that our demand model of ARD increases awareness and discussion around the importance of cognitive demands for maintaining cognitive function throughout the lifespan. Given that the maintenance of a cognitively active lifestyle has such tremendous promise with virtually zero risks, it is our hope that this model will inspire an enhanced effort to make it a cornerstone of dementia prevention and clinical care, along with research programs to characterize the activities of greatest benefit more thoroughly. Even if proven wrong, we at least hope that people are encouraged in the meantime to dance and sing, and perform more of the critical activities that make us uniquely human, including doing so in social and medical groups with facilitated and peer support.<span><sup>59, 60</sup></span> In the broader sense, this is one of the clearest reasons to implement lifestyle medicine, even if all the mechanistic details remain to be elucidated.</p><p>Dr. Turknett is the founder of Brainjo, a company that creates educational programs for adult learners, and President of Physicians for Ancestral Health. Dr. Wood is a paid scientific advisor for Hintsa Performance, Sidekick Health, Thriva LLC, and Rewire Fitness, and is a founding trustee of the British Society of Lifestyle Medicine.</p><p>The authors received no specific funding for this work.</p>","PeriodicalId":74076,"journal":{"name":"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2022-10-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/lim2.70","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Lifestyle medicine (Hoboken, N.J.)","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/lim2.70","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"Medicine","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

With the general trend of increasing lifespan alongside population-level success in reducing the mortality from cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases, the population burden of Alzheimer's disease (AD) has steadily accelerated. In the United States, the mortality rate due to AD has increased from less than 0.5 per 100,000 in 1980 to approximately 30 per 100,000 in 2019.1 Worldwide, the number of individuals with dementia is expected to at least triple by 2050,2 with some reports suggesting that AD has the potential to bankrupt healthcare systems.3 At the same time, the development of, and investment in, multiple pharmacological agents directed toward modifying the pathological “hallmarks” of AD have yielded disappointing results. Existing pharmaceuticals offer modest symptomatic benefits, at best, without modifying the course of the disease. Taken together, these factors highlight the urgent need for a critical reappraisal of the underlying risk factors for AD and potential interventions.

In an attempt to reframe potential preventative and therapeutic approaches to AD, we recently proposed a model that suggests demand–function coupling in the brain is the critical upstream factor driving long-term cognitive function.4 In this model, we describe how the health and function of any tissue, including the brain, is shaped by the demands placed upon it. In the setting of increased demand, demand–function coupling drives increases in growth and function, but also upregulates processes of cellular repair and regeneration. The result is a tissue that is more resilient, plastic, and with a greater capacity for increased work output in the face of increased requirements. In this way, we propose that the structure and function of the brain are driven by the demands placed upon it, much as cardiac and skeletal muscles respond to exercise. And as muscle or cardiovascular function decline with bed rest or detraining, the structure and function of the brain decline in a coupled manner when adequate demands are not placed upon it. Cognitive decline is then essentially an expression of “frailty” of the brain - defined as lacking additional capacity to function above basic requirements - as a result of reduced demand. Although it may appear subtle, this reframing of the cascade may be critical in understanding the disease process and intervening as healthcare providers. Whereas the prevailing explanations to date have described cognitive activity as a mitigating force; in this model, we propose that cognitive demand instead impacts the primary pathogenetic process. In fact, given the established phenomenon of demand coupling in neural tissue, we believe that this model provides the most parsimonious account of disease pathogenesis.

In order to explain the approach, we first made two suggestions for a cognitive framework that we believe are essential if we are to better understand the mechanistic underpinnings of dementia and attempt to intervene in the disease process. The first suggestion was that we should separate late-onset or “sporadic” AD from early-onset familial AD.4 Though they both have historically been labeled as forms of AD due to shared neuropathological hallmarks at autopsy, these are completely different diseases with respect to their risk factors and disease course. As such, our model only discusses late-onset AD, which we call age-related dementia (ARD), which is preceded by a period of age-related cognitive decline (ARCD).

The second suggestion was that we should take an opportunity to explore ARD etiology and intervention points from a different level of biological organization. Every biological phenomenon can be described and characterized at multiple levels of biological organization, from the level of cells and molecules to that of populations and ecosystems.5 Every disease phenomenon therefore has a level of analysis and characterization best suited to a coherent explanation of pathogenesis, which is typically the level most upstream in the causal chain. Characterizations of downstream consequences at lower levels of organization, regardless of their level of detail, do not necessarily confer additional or greater explanatory power. In the case of ARD, the field has generally focused on granular, lower level molecular and neuropathological details. This is despite the fact that a growing body of evidence indicates that lifestyle and environment—representing the organismal or societal level of biological organization—are the ultimate level of causation for ARD. If this is truly the case, then interventions at the organismal level are those that are likely to be most impactful, also explaining the general failure (to date) of interventions targeted at the molecular level of AD and ARD.

One issue with changing the level of organization at which we intervene in a disease process is the fact that it requires us to divest ourselves from thinking purely in terms of well-defined biochemical mechanisms. When developing pharmaceutical agents, it is critical to target a single mechanism while minimizing off-target effects. By comparison, when intervening at the organismal level an intervention may be able to generate powerful results without ever having complete knowledge of its biochemical effects. Indeed, the lack of one single mechanism at the molecular or cellular level of organization has seemed to be a fundamental problem when trying to institute lifestyle medicine at the societal level within medical systems that are singularly focused on the mechanism of action of a given intervention. A good example of this is physical activity, which is perhaps the most impactful single intervention for the treatment and prevention of noncommunicable disease.6 Increases in exercise and non-exercise physical activities are uniformly recommended by all government, non-governmental, and health-related organizations despite the fact we are still not entirely sure how physical activity “works.” A number of mechanisms linking physical activity and health, including both acute and chronic functional adaptations to exercise, have been elucidated and documented over several decades. However, even in recent years, we have continued to discover mechanisms by which exercise modulates whole-body health and function, including novel pathways involving irisin, MOTS-c (mitochondrial open reading frame of the 12S rRNA type-c), and Lac-Phe (N-lactoyl-phenylalanine) as just a few examples published in high-impact journals with a large amount of associated press and media interest.7-9 If we accept that organismal-level interventions with pleiotropic (and sometimes not fully understood) effects may have the greatest benefit on certain aspects of human health, this allows us to reframe our approach to diseases where few inroads have been made so far, such as ARD.

When discussing the demand model of ARD with clinicians and researchers with expertise in dementia and neurogenerative conditions, it has not been uncommon for them to ask about other established risk factors, particularly those related to general health and the environment. We must therefore make it very clear that we believe that the environmental component of ARCD and ARD is well-established,10-14 and should be an important part of any model for disease etiology or intervention. This includes evidence for significantly increased risk of dementia as well as worse cognitive function associated with reduced physical activity,10, 15-17 poor sleep,10, 18, 19 metabolic disease,20, 21 nutrient deficiencies,14, 22 hormonal insufficiency,23 altered body composition,24-27 and social isolation or poor social support.28 Early evidence is also mounting to suggest that lifestyle and environmental modification has the potential to prevent or even reverse ARD, especially when initiated early in the disease process.14, 29, 30 A prime example of this is the pioneering work by Prof. David Smith and colleagues, who showed that intervention with B vitamins to lower elevated homocysteine slowed the rate of brain atrophy and cognitive decline, particularly in the setting of adequate omega-3 fatty acid status.22 Importantly, the environmental influences on ARD risk map directly to the six evidence-based pillars of lifestyle medicine (Figure 1). We therefore caution against thinking of factors as mutually exclusive, and instead believe the above lifestyle end environmental factors to be critical components required for optimal demand–function coupling in the brain. In order to adapt to increased demand in a tissue, that tissue must also have adequate nutrient availability, hormonal and trophic support, and vascular supply, as well as the absence of toxic exposures and opportunity for sleep recovery and adaptation (e.g., sleep). Insufficiencies in any one of these factors would likely undermine the health-promoting adaptations to cognitive demand. To return to the comparison to exercise, an individual may have all the components required to build healthy muscle tissue—nutrition, hormonal status, sleep, etc.—but in the absence of physical exercise they will not improve their fitness. We believe that all the evidence points to brain function, unsurprisingly, being the same.

To move beyond the hypothetical, what is the evidence for the primary importance of cognitive demand and what might that mean for the future of AD research and intervention? A recent meta-analysis by Yu et al.14 found that cognitive demand late in life was the single most protective factor against AD. Similarly, multiple epidemiological studies suggest that the loss of work-related cognitive stimulus due to retirement results in a parallel increase in the risk of cognitive decline, even after adjusting for confounders that might explain both early retirement and cognitive decline such as comorbid medical conditions.31-33 The negative effect of loss of cognitive stimulus is also reversible, as evidenced by increased risk of cognitive decline in those that lose sensory function (e.g., sight, smell, or hearing), which can be overcome by interventions that restore the impaired sense.34-38 Other types of beneficial cognitive demand including bilingualism or playing a musical instrument, with greater benefits on brain structure in those for whom the practice is more challenging.39-45 Coordinative exercise (e.g., yoga, tai chi, or dancing) also appears to have greater benefits on brain structure and function compared to intensity-matched exercise without a coordination component.46, 47 Even structured brain training can have carryover to real-world improvements in memory and executive functions.39, 48-52 Although they may initially appear disparate, the upshot of these strands of evidence is that an individual may be able to pick any skill or challenge they are particularly interested in as a way to increase cognitive demands. This would also increase both enjoyment and compliance, as well as making it more likely that these skills may be applied in a social or group setting, which would expectedly provide additional benefits.53

We must acknowledge that limitations exist with the current model and there remains much work to be done in order to determine whether it might help shape the future of interventions for ARD. Cognitive demand is difficult to quantify, and therefore the model is difficult to test. From an implementation standpoint, one might hope that better public awareness of the importance of cognitive demand could drive behavior change and increased cognitive demand at the individual level; however, similar approaches to physical activity have yet to be broadly successful and personalized interventions are likely to be needed.54 It is also not yet known whether cognitive demand is both necessary and sufficient to explain ARCD and ARD, and any current attempt to prevent or reverse cognitive decline in a holistic manner should include multi-modal interventions such as those employed in the FINGER (Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability) trial.55 This hampers the ability to dissect the relevant contributions of different lifestyle factors, including cognitive demand, on the risk of ARCD and ARD. However, novel trial methodologies could be employed to sequentially alter individual risk factors to determine the relative contributions of cognitive demand.4, 56, 57 Additional work could also be done in preclinical experiments such as in rodent models. For instance, we could examine whether the brain is still able to beneficially adapt to increased cognitive demands in the setting of suboptimal nutrient status, general health, or with the presence of toxic exposures such as particulate atmospheric pollution. As an example, work in a mouse model of elevated homocysteine—an important risk factor for AD in humans as mentioned above—found that increased cognitive demand through environmental enrichment was still able to result in beneficial trophic responses in the brain.58 If this was the case in a broad range of models of the environmental risk factors for AD in humans, then that would support the proposed model.

Although there is clearly much to be done, we hope that our demand model of ARD increases awareness and discussion around the importance of cognitive demands for maintaining cognitive function throughout the lifespan. Given that the maintenance of a cognitively active lifestyle has such tremendous promise with virtually zero risks, it is our hope that this model will inspire an enhanced effort to make it a cornerstone of dementia prevention and clinical care, along with research programs to characterize the activities of greatest benefit more thoroughly. Even if proven wrong, we at least hope that people are encouraged in the meantime to dance and sing, and perform more of the critical activities that make us uniquely human, including doing so in social and medical groups with facilitated and peer support.59, 60 In the broader sense, this is one of the clearest reasons to implement lifestyle medicine, even if all the mechanistic details remain to be elucidated.

Dr. Turknett is the founder of Brainjo, a company that creates educational programs for adult learners, and President of Physicians for Ancestral Health. Dr. Wood is a paid scientific advisor for Hintsa Performance, Sidekick Health, Thriva LLC, and Rewire Fitness, and is a founding trustee of the British Society of Lifestyle Medicine.

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

Abstract Image

缺乏适当的认知需求是痴呆风险的主要驱动因素吗?
随着人类寿命不断延长的大趋势以及人口水平在降低心脑血管疾病死亡率方面的成功,阿尔茨海默病(AD)的人口负担稳步增加。在美国,阿尔茨海默病的死亡率从1980年的每10万人不到0.5人增加到2019年的每10万人约30人。1在世界范围内,痴呆症患者的数量预计到2050年将至少增加两倍,2一些报告表明,阿尔茨海默病有可能使医疗系统破产与此同时,针对改变阿尔茨海默病病理“特征”的多种药物的开发和投资却产生了令人失望的结果。现有的药物最多只能提供适度的对症治疗,而不能改变疾病的进程。综上所述,这些因素突出了迫切需要对阿尔茨海默病的潜在危险因素和潜在干预措施进行关键的重新评估。为了重新构建AD的潜在预防和治疗方法,我们最近提出了一个模型,该模型表明大脑中的需求-功能耦合是驱动长期认知功能的关键上游因素在这个模型中,我们描述了包括大脑在内的任何组织的健康和功能是如何被施加在它身上的需求所塑造的。在需求增加的背景下,需求-功能耦合驱动生长和功能的增加,但也上调细胞修复和再生过程。其结果是一种更有弹性、可塑性的组织,在面对增加的需求时,具有更大的工作输出能力。通过这种方式,我们提出大脑的结构和功能是由施加在它身上的需求驱动的,就像心脏和骨骼肌对运动的反应一样。当肌肉或心血管功能随着卧床休息或去训练而下降时,当没有适当的需求时,大脑的结构和功能也会以一种耦合的方式下降。因此,认知能力下降本质上是大脑“脆弱”的一种表现——定义为缺乏超出基本需求的额外功能——这是需求减少的结果。虽然它可能看起来很微妙,但这种级联的重构可能对理解疾病过程和作为医疗保健提供者进行干预至关重要。鉴于迄今为止的主流解释将认知活动描述为一种缓解力量;在这个模型中,我们提出认知需求反而影响了原发性发病过程。事实上,考虑到神经组织中需求耦合的既定现象,我们认为该模型提供了疾病发病机制的最简洁的解释。为了解释这种方法,我们首先对认知框架提出了两个建议,我们认为如果我们要更好地理解痴呆症的机制基础并试图干预疾病过程,这两个框架是必不可少的。第一个建议是,我们应该将迟发性或“散发性”AD与早发性家族性AD区分开来。尽管在历史上,由于在尸检中有共同的神经病理特征,它们都被标记为AD的形式,但就其危险因素和病程而言,它们是完全不同的疾病。因此,我们的模型只讨论了晚发性阿尔茨海默病,我们称之为年龄相关性痴呆(ARD),在此之前会出现一段与年龄相关的认知衰退(ARCD)。第二个建议是,我们应该抓住机会从不同的生物组织水平探索ARD的病因和干预点。每一种生物现象都可以在生物组织的多个层面上进行描述和表征,从细胞和分子的层面到种群和生态系统的层面因此,每种疾病现象都有一个最适合连贯解释发病机制的分析和表征水平,这通常是因果链中最上游的水平。在较低层次的组织中对下游结果的描述,不管其详细程度如何,都不一定赋予额外的或更大的解释力。在ARD的情况下,该领域通常集中在颗粒,较低水平的分子和神经病理细节。尽管有越来越多的证据表明,生活方式和环境——代表生物组织的有机体或社会层面——是导致ARD的最终原因。如果情况确实如此,那么机体水平的干预措施可能是最有效的,这也解释了(迄今为止)针对AD和ARD的分子水平干预措施的普遍失败。改变我们干预疾病过程的组织水平的一个问题是,它要求我们放弃纯粹从定义良好的生化机制的角度来思考。 在开发药物制剂时,关键是要针对单一机制,同时尽量减少脱靶效应。相比之下,当在有机体层面进行干预时,干预可能会产生强大的结果,而无需完全了解其生化效应。事实上,当试图在医疗系统的社会层面建立生活方式医学时,在组织的分子或细胞水平上缺乏单一的机制似乎是一个根本问题,因为医疗系统只关注特定干预的作用机制。这方面的一个很好的例子是身体活动,它可能是治疗和预防非传染性疾病的最有效的单一干预措施尽管我们仍然不能完全确定体育活动是如何“起作用”的,但所有政府、非政府组织和健康相关组织都一致建议增加锻炼和非锻炼性体育活动。几十年来,许多将身体活动与健康联系起来的机制,包括对运动的急性和慢性功能适应,已经得到阐明和记录。然而,即使在最近几年,我们继续发现运动调节全身健康和功能的机制,包括涉及鸢尾素、MOTS-c (12S rRNA型c的线粒体开放阅读框架)和Lac-Phe (n -乳酸基苯丙氨酸)的新途径,这只是在高影响力期刊上发表的几个例子,引起了大量相关媒体和媒体的兴趣。7-9如果我们接受具有多效性(有时尚未完全理解)效果的机体层面干预可能对人类健康的某些方面有最大的益处,这将使我们能够重新构建迄今为止尚未取得进展的疾病的方法,例如ARD。在与临床医生和研究人员讨论ARD的需求模型时,他们通常会询问其他已确定的风险因素,特别是与一般健康和环境相关的风险因素。因此,我们必须非常清楚地表明,我们认为ARCD和ARD的环境成分是完善的,10-14,应该是任何疾病病因学或干预模型的重要组成部分。这包括与身体活动减少、10,15 -17睡眠不足、10,18,19代谢疾病、20,21营养缺乏、14,22激素不足、23身体组成改变、24-27和社会孤立或社会支持不足相关的痴呆症风险显著增加的证据越来越多的早期证据表明,生活方式和环境的改变有可能预防甚至逆转ARD,特别是在疾病过程的早期开始。14,29,30这方面的一个主要例子是David Smith教授及其同事的开创性工作,他们表明,用B族维生素来降低高同型半胱氨酸的干预可以减缓脑萎缩和认知能力下降的速度,特别是在omega-3脂肪酸充足的情况下重要的是,环境对ARD风险的影响直接映射到生活方式医学的六个基于证据的支柱(图1)。因此,我们警告不要认为因素是相互排斥的,相反,我们认为上述生活方式和环境因素是大脑中最佳需求-功能耦合所需的关键组成部分。为了适应组织中增加的需求,该组织还必须有足够的营养供应,激素和营养支持,血管供应,以及缺乏有毒物质暴露和睡眠恢复和适应的机会(例如睡眠)。这些因素中的任何一个不足都可能破坏对认知需求的健康促进适应。回到与运动的比较上来,一个人可能拥有建立健康肌肉组织所需的所有要素——营养、荷尔蒙状态、睡眠等——但如果缺乏体育锻炼,他们就不会提高健康水平。我们相信所有的证据都指向大脑功能,毫不奇怪,是一样的。超越假设,认知需求的首要重要性的证据是什么,这对未来的AD研究和干预可能意味着什么?Yu等人最近的一项荟萃分析发现,晚年的认知需求是对抗AD的唯一最具保护作用的因素。同样,多项流行病学研究表明,退休导致的与工作相关的认知刺激的丧失导致认知能力下降的风险同时增加,即使在调整了可能解释提前退休和认知能力下降的混杂因素(如合并症)之后也是如此。 31-33丧失认知刺激的负面影响也是可逆的,丧失感觉功能(如视觉、嗅觉或听觉)的人认知能力下降的风险增加就证明了这一点,这可以通过恢复受损感觉的干预措施来克服。34-38其他类型的有益认知需求包括双语能力或演奏乐器,对那些练习更具挑战性的人的大脑结构有更大的好处。39-45与没有协调成分的高强度运动相比,协调性运动(如瑜伽、太极或舞蹈)似乎对大脑结构和功能有更大的好处。46,47即使是结构化的大脑训练也可以延续到现实生活中记忆和执行功能的改善。39,48 -52尽管它们最初看起来是不同的,但这些证据的结果是,一个人可以选择任何他们特别感兴趣的技能或挑战,作为增加认知需求的一种方式。这也将增加乐趣和依从性,并使这些技能更有可能在社交或团体环境中应用,这将提供额外的好处。53我们必须承认,目前的模式存在局限性,为了确定它是否有助于塑造ARD干预措施的未来,仍有许多工作要做。认知需求难以量化,因此模型难以检验。从实施的角度来看,人们可能希望公众更好地认识到认知需求的重要性,从而推动个人层面的行为改变和认知需求的增加;然而,类似的体育锻炼方法尚未取得广泛成功,可能需要个性化的干预目前还不清楚认知需求是否足以解释ARCD和ARD,目前任何以整体方式预防或逆转认知衰退的尝试都应该包括多模式干预,如芬兰老年预防认知障碍和残疾干预研究(FINGER)试验中采用的干预措施这妨碍了分析不同生活方式因素(包括认知需求)对ARCD和ARD风险的相关贡献。然而,可以采用新的试验方法依次改变个体风险因素,以确定认知需求的相对贡献。4,56,57还可以在临床前实验(如啮齿动物模型)中进行额外的工作。例如,我们可以检查大脑是否仍然能够在营养状况不佳的情况下,在一般健康状况下,或者在有毒物质(如大气颗粒污染)存在的情况下,有益地适应不断增加的认知需求。例如,在同型半胱氨酸升高的小鼠模型中(如上所述,同型半胱氨酸是人类阿尔茨海默病的一个重要危险因素)的研究发现,通过环境富集而增加的认知需求仍然能够在大脑中产生有益的营养反应如果在人类AD的环境风险因素的广泛模型中也是如此,那么这将支持所提出的模型。虽然显然还有很多工作要做,但我们希望我们的ARD需求模型能够提高人们对认知需求在整个生命周期中维持认知功能重要性的认识和讨论。考虑到保持积极的认知生活方式有如此巨大的希望,几乎没有风险,我们希望这种模式能激发人们的更多努力,使其成为痴呆症预防和临床护理的基石,同时研究项目也能更彻底地描述最有益的活动。即使被证明是错误的,我们至少希望人们在此期间被鼓励跳舞和唱歌,并进行更多使我们成为独特人类的关键活动,包括在有便利和同伴支持的社会和医疗团体中这样做。59,60从更广泛的意义上说,这是实施生活方式医学的最明确的理由之一,即使所有的机械细节仍有待阐明。特克内特是Brainjo的创始人,这是一家为成人学习者创建教育项目的公司,也是祖传健康医师协会的主席。他是Hintsa Performance, Sidekick Health, Thriva LLC和Rewire Fitness的有偿科学顾问,也是英国生活方式医学协会的创始受托人。作者没有得到这项工作的特别资助。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
2.10
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
审稿时长
7 weeks
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信