Using Narratives to Assess Bilingual Children

Q4 Social Sciences
C. Westby
{"title":"Using Narratives to Assess Bilingual Children","authors":"C. Westby","doi":"10.1177/10483950221101453","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Identifying a language difference versus a language disorder in young bilingual children is a challenging task for speech-language pathologists. Narrative assessments are increasingly employed in identifying bilingual students with language impairment. Research on dual language learners (DLLs) has shown that children achieve language competency in their second language (L2) first in narrative organization (macrostructure), then vocabulary, and last morphology (Paradis, 2016). Narrative tasks are often used to assess the language of schoolage children because they tap a range of functional language skills (as opposed to decontextualized language assessments that, for example, ask children to name pictures, point to pictures, and finish sentences). Narratives require a variety of receptive/comprehension language skills and expressive/production language skills. Children must be able to remember a story they hear and understand who the story characters were, what they did, and why; they must also be able to produce stories in which they coherently link ideas and organize those ideas around a common theme (Gillam & Pearson, 2004). Difficulty in performing these tasks is associated with language impairment (Tsimpli et al., 2016). A significant discrepancy between receptive and expressive ability has been a hallmark of language impairment (Gibson et al., 2018). Recent research indicates that a receptive–expressive gap occurs in vocabulary and semantic testing for bilingual children (Gibson et al., 2012, 2014b), but not with typically developing (TD) children; furthermore, the discrepancy is exacerbated for bilingual children with primary language impairment (PLI; Gibson et al., 2014a). Such a discrepancy would not be predicted in theory because standardized tests are designed based on normative research to yield outcomes that should be comparable. A newsletter dedicated to speech & language in school-age children","PeriodicalId":39491,"journal":{"name":"Word of Mouth","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2022-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Word of Mouth","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10483950221101453","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"Social Sciences","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Identifying a language difference versus a language disorder in young bilingual children is a challenging task for speech-language pathologists. Narrative assessments are increasingly employed in identifying bilingual students with language impairment. Research on dual language learners (DLLs) has shown that children achieve language competency in their second language (L2) first in narrative organization (macrostructure), then vocabulary, and last morphology (Paradis, 2016). Narrative tasks are often used to assess the language of schoolage children because they tap a range of functional language skills (as opposed to decontextualized language assessments that, for example, ask children to name pictures, point to pictures, and finish sentences). Narratives require a variety of receptive/comprehension language skills and expressive/production language skills. Children must be able to remember a story they hear and understand who the story characters were, what they did, and why; they must also be able to produce stories in which they coherently link ideas and organize those ideas around a common theme (Gillam & Pearson, 2004). Difficulty in performing these tasks is associated with language impairment (Tsimpli et al., 2016). A significant discrepancy between receptive and expressive ability has been a hallmark of language impairment (Gibson et al., 2018). Recent research indicates that a receptive–expressive gap occurs in vocabulary and semantic testing for bilingual children (Gibson et al., 2012, 2014b), but not with typically developing (TD) children; furthermore, the discrepancy is exacerbated for bilingual children with primary language impairment (PLI; Gibson et al., 2014a). Such a discrepancy would not be predicted in theory because standardized tests are designed based on normative research to yield outcomes that should be comparable. A newsletter dedicated to speech & language in school-age children
用叙事来评估双语儿童
识别年轻双语儿童的语言差异和语言障碍对言语语言病理学家来说是一项具有挑战性的任务。叙事评估越来越多地用于识别有语言障碍的双语学生。对双语学习者(DLL)的研究表明,儿童在第二语言(L2)中首先在叙事组织(宏观结构)、词汇和最后的形态方面获得了语言能力(Paradis,2016)。叙事任务通常用于评估学龄儿童的语言,因为它们利用了一系列功能性语言技能(而不是去文本化的语言评估,例如,要求儿童命名图片、指向图片和完成句子)。叙述需要多种接受/理解语言技能和表达/生产语言技能。孩子们必须能够记住他们听到的故事,并理解故事中的人物是谁,他们做了什么,以及为什么;他们还必须能够制作出连贯地将想法联系起来并围绕共同主题组织这些想法的故事(Gillam&Pearson,2004)。执行这些任务的困难与语言障碍有关(Tsimpli等人,2016)。接受能力和表达能力之间的显著差异是语言障碍的标志(Gibson et al.,2018)。最近的研究表明,双语儿童在词汇和语义测试中存在接受-表达差距(Gibson et al.,20122014b),但在典型发育(TD)儿童中没有;此外,对于有初级语言障碍的双语儿童来说,这种差异更加严重(PLI;Gibson等人,2014a)。这种差异在理论上是无法预测的,因为标准化测试是基于规范性研究设计的,目的是产生具有可比性的结果。一份致力于学龄儿童言语和语言的时事通讯
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Word of Mouth
Word of Mouth Social Sciences-Education
CiteScore
0.10
自引率
0.00%
发文量
27
期刊介绍: ...helps frontline clinicians keep up with the latest trends in working with school-age children. Each 16-page issue of bare-bones, down-to-earth information includes reviews, resources, idea swap, and short bits.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信