The emergence of verse templates through iterated learning

IF 2.1 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS
V. deCastro-Arrazola, S. Kirby
{"title":"The emergence of verse templates through iterated learning","authors":"V. deCastro-Arrazola, S. Kirby","doi":"10.1093/JOLE/LZY013","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Every language produces some type of verse in the form of songs, poems or nursery rhymes, which can be analysed as a layer of words set to a template (e.g. a tune, a poetic metre). Verse templates typically consist of hierarchically organised sections: songs are made up of stanzas, divided into lines, containing bars, etc. We hypothesise that this kind of patterns may emerge in the process of cultural transmission; unstructured sound sequences impose a challenge to short-termmemory, but chunking the input makes it easier to parse and reproduce the sequences accurately. In order to test this hypothesis, we have run an iterated learning experiment where random sequences of syllables are evolved across four transmission chains with ten generations of subjects each (all native Dutch speakers). The initial random sequences are generated by concatenating twelve tokens of the set {ban, bi, ta, tin}, as a way to materialise the abstract verse templates without using content-words. More precisely, the experiment aims to model the sequences of nonsense syllables used in many traditions to communicate the rhythmic patterns underlying songs (e.g. bols in Hindustani music, lalay patterns in Berber verse). Participants listened to the sequences of syllables, and tried to reproduce them using four computer keys, eachmapped to one of the four syllables used in the input sequences. The relative timing of the participants’ responses were normalised so that the input always consisted of completely isochronous sequences. Overall, the results show that sequences become shorter, easier to recall and more structured in the transmission process. Some regularities can be related to a global tendency to chunk the input and increase the popularity of a handful of ngrams. Besides, sequences increasingly tend to be opened by a heavy syllable (e.g. ban) and closed by a light syllable (e.g. ta), which can derive from a Dutch-specific bias.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1000,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/JOLE/LZY013","citationCount":"8","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Language Evolution","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1093/JOLE/LZY013","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"0","JCRName":"LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8

Abstract

Every language produces some type of verse in the form of songs, poems or nursery rhymes, which can be analysed as a layer of words set to a template (e.g. a tune, a poetic metre). Verse templates typically consist of hierarchically organised sections: songs are made up of stanzas, divided into lines, containing bars, etc. We hypothesise that this kind of patterns may emerge in the process of cultural transmission; unstructured sound sequences impose a challenge to short-termmemory, but chunking the input makes it easier to parse and reproduce the sequences accurately. In order to test this hypothesis, we have run an iterated learning experiment where random sequences of syllables are evolved across four transmission chains with ten generations of subjects each (all native Dutch speakers). The initial random sequences are generated by concatenating twelve tokens of the set {ban, bi, ta, tin}, as a way to materialise the abstract verse templates without using content-words. More precisely, the experiment aims to model the sequences of nonsense syllables used in many traditions to communicate the rhythmic patterns underlying songs (e.g. bols in Hindustani music, lalay patterns in Berber verse). Participants listened to the sequences of syllables, and tried to reproduce them using four computer keys, eachmapped to one of the four syllables used in the input sequences. The relative timing of the participants’ responses were normalised so that the input always consisted of completely isochronous sequences. Overall, the results show that sequences become shorter, easier to recall and more structured in the transmission process. Some regularities can be related to a global tendency to chunk the input and increase the popularity of a handful of ngrams. Besides, sequences increasingly tend to be opened by a heavy syllable (e.g. ban) and closed by a light syllable (e.g. ta), which can derive from a Dutch-specific bias.
通过迭代学习出现诗歌模板
每种语言都会以歌曲、诗歌或儿歌的形式产生某种类型的诗歌,这些诗歌可以作为一层设置在模板上的单词来分析(例如曲调、韵律)。诗歌模板通常由分层组织的部分组成:歌曲由诗节组成,分为几行,包含小节等。我们假设这种模式可能在文化传播过程中出现;非结构化的声音序列对短期记忆提出了挑战,但对输入进行分块可以更容易地准确解析和再现序列。为了验证这一假设,我们进行了一项迭代学习实验,在该实验中,音节的随机序列在四个传输链上进化,每个传输链有十代受试者(均为母语为荷兰语的人)。初始随机序列是通过连接集合{ban,bi,ta,tin}的十二个标记来生成的,作为一种在不使用内容词的情况下实现抽象诗歌模板的方式。更准确地说,该实验旨在模拟许多传统中用于传达歌曲节奏模式的无意义音节序列(例如,印度斯坦音乐中的bols,柏柏尔诗歌中的lalay模式)。参与者听音节序列,并试图使用四个计算机键复制它们,每个键都映射到输入序列中使用的四个音节中的一个。参与者反应的相对时间被归一化,使得输入总是由完全等时序列组成。总体而言,结果表明,序列在传输过程中变得更短、更容易回忆和更结构化。一些规律可能与整体趋势有关,即对输入进行分组并增加少数ngram的受欢迎程度。此外,序列越来越倾向于由重音节(如ban)打开,由轻音节(如ta)关闭,这可能源于荷兰语特有的偏见。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Journal of Language Evolution
Journal of Language Evolution Social Sciences-Linguistics and Language
CiteScore
4.50
自引率
7.70%
发文量
8
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信