Women in Academic Science: A Changing Landscape.

1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology
Stephen J Ceci, Donna K Ginther, Shulamit Kahn, Wendy M Williams
{"title":"Women in Academic Science: A Changing Landscape.","authors":"Stephen J Ceci,&nbsp;Donna K Ginther,&nbsp;Shulamit Kahn,&nbsp;Wendy M Williams","doi":"10.1177/1529100614541236","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Much has been written in the past two decades about women in academic science careers, but this literature is contradictory. Many analyses have revealed a level playing field, with men and women faring equally, whereas other analyses have suggested numerous areas in which the playing field is not level. The only widely-agreed-upon conclusion is that women are underrepresented in college majors, graduate school programs, and the professoriate in those fields that are the most mathematically intensive, such as geoscience, engineering, economics, mathematics/computer science, and the physical sciences. In other scientific fields (psychology, life science, social science), women are found in much higher percentages. In this monograph, we undertake extensive life-course analyses comparing the trajectories of women and men in math-intensive fields with those of their counterparts in non-math-intensive fields in which women are close to parity with or even exceed the number of men. We begin by examining early-childhood differences in spatial processing and follow this through quantitative performance in middle childhood and adolescence, including high school coursework. We then focus on the transition of the sexes from high school to college major, then to graduate school, and, finally, to careers in academic science. The results of our myriad analyses reveal that early sex differences in spatial and mathematical reasoning need not stem from biological bases, that the gap between average female and male math ability is narrowing (suggesting strong environmental influences), and that sex differences in math ability at the right tail show variation over time and across nationalities, ethnicities, and other factors, indicating that the ratio of males to females at the right tail can and does change. We find that gender differences in attitudes toward and expectations about math careers and ability (controlling for actual ability) are evident by kindergarten and increase thereafter, leading to lower female propensities to major in math-intensive subjects in college but higher female propensities to major in non-math-intensive sciences, with overall science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) majors at 50% female for more than a decade. Post-college, although men with majors in math-intensive subjects have historically chosen and completed PhDs in these fields more often than women, the gap has recently narrowed by two thirds; among non-math-intensive STEM majors, women are more likely than men to go into health and other people-related occupations instead of pursuing PhDs. Importantly, of those who obtain doctorates in math-intensive fields, men and women entering the professoriate have equivalent access to tenure-track academic jobs in science, and they persist and are remunerated at comparable rates-with some caveats that we discuss. The transition from graduate programs to assistant professorships shows more pipeline leakage in the fields in which women are already very prevalent (psychology, life science, social science) than in the math-intensive fields in which they are underrepresented but in which the number of females holding assistant professorships is at least commensurate with (if not greater than) that of males. That is, invitations to interview for tenure-track positions in math-intensive fields-as well as actual employment offers-reveal that female PhD applicants fare at least as well as their male counterparts in math-intensive fields. Along these same lines, our analyses reveal that manuscript reviewing and grant funding are gender neutral: Male and female authors and principal investigators are equally likely to have their manuscripts accepted by journal editors and their grants funded, with only very occasional exceptions. There are no compelling sex differences in hours worked or average citations per publication, but there is an overall male advantage in productivity. We attempt to reconcile these results amid the disparate claims made regarding their causes, examining sex differences in citations, hours worked, and interests. We conclude by suggesting that although in the past, gender discrimination was an important cause of women's underrepresentation in scientific academic careers, this claim has continued to be invoked after it has ceased being a valid cause of women's underrepresentation in math-intensive fields. Consequently, current barriers to women's full participation in mathematically intensive academic science fields are rooted in pre-college factors and the subsequent likelihood of majoring in these fields, and future research should focus on these barriers rather than misdirecting attention toward historical barriers that no longer account for women's underrepresentation in academic science. </p>","PeriodicalId":37882,"journal":{"name":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","volume":"15 3","pages":"75-141"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2014-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1529100614541236","citationCount":"595","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100614541236","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"Psychology","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 595

Abstract

Much has been written in the past two decades about women in academic science careers, but this literature is contradictory. Many analyses have revealed a level playing field, with men and women faring equally, whereas other analyses have suggested numerous areas in which the playing field is not level. The only widely-agreed-upon conclusion is that women are underrepresented in college majors, graduate school programs, and the professoriate in those fields that are the most mathematically intensive, such as geoscience, engineering, economics, mathematics/computer science, and the physical sciences. In other scientific fields (psychology, life science, social science), women are found in much higher percentages. In this monograph, we undertake extensive life-course analyses comparing the trajectories of women and men in math-intensive fields with those of their counterparts in non-math-intensive fields in which women are close to parity with or even exceed the number of men. We begin by examining early-childhood differences in spatial processing and follow this through quantitative performance in middle childhood and adolescence, including high school coursework. We then focus on the transition of the sexes from high school to college major, then to graduate school, and, finally, to careers in academic science. The results of our myriad analyses reveal that early sex differences in spatial and mathematical reasoning need not stem from biological bases, that the gap between average female and male math ability is narrowing (suggesting strong environmental influences), and that sex differences in math ability at the right tail show variation over time and across nationalities, ethnicities, and other factors, indicating that the ratio of males to females at the right tail can and does change. We find that gender differences in attitudes toward and expectations about math careers and ability (controlling for actual ability) are evident by kindergarten and increase thereafter, leading to lower female propensities to major in math-intensive subjects in college but higher female propensities to major in non-math-intensive sciences, with overall science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) majors at 50% female for more than a decade. Post-college, although men with majors in math-intensive subjects have historically chosen and completed PhDs in these fields more often than women, the gap has recently narrowed by two thirds; among non-math-intensive STEM majors, women are more likely than men to go into health and other people-related occupations instead of pursuing PhDs. Importantly, of those who obtain doctorates in math-intensive fields, men and women entering the professoriate have equivalent access to tenure-track academic jobs in science, and they persist and are remunerated at comparable rates-with some caveats that we discuss. The transition from graduate programs to assistant professorships shows more pipeline leakage in the fields in which women are already very prevalent (psychology, life science, social science) than in the math-intensive fields in which they are underrepresented but in which the number of females holding assistant professorships is at least commensurate with (if not greater than) that of males. That is, invitations to interview for tenure-track positions in math-intensive fields-as well as actual employment offers-reveal that female PhD applicants fare at least as well as their male counterparts in math-intensive fields. Along these same lines, our analyses reveal that manuscript reviewing and grant funding are gender neutral: Male and female authors and principal investigators are equally likely to have their manuscripts accepted by journal editors and their grants funded, with only very occasional exceptions. There are no compelling sex differences in hours worked or average citations per publication, but there is an overall male advantage in productivity. We attempt to reconcile these results amid the disparate claims made regarding their causes, examining sex differences in citations, hours worked, and interests. We conclude by suggesting that although in the past, gender discrimination was an important cause of women's underrepresentation in scientific academic careers, this claim has continued to be invoked after it has ceased being a valid cause of women's underrepresentation in math-intensive fields. Consequently, current barriers to women's full participation in mathematically intensive academic science fields are rooted in pre-college factors and the subsequent likelihood of majoring in these fields, and future research should focus on these barriers rather than misdirecting attention toward historical barriers that no longer account for women's underrepresentation in academic science.

学术科学中的女性:不断变化的景观。
在过去的二十年里,有很多关于女性从事学术科学事业的文章,但这些文献是矛盾的。许多分析揭示了一个公平的竞争环境,男女平等,而其他分析则表明,在许多领域,竞争环境并不公平。唯一得到广泛认同的结论是,女性在大学专业、研究生课程和数学最密集的领域(如地球科学、工程学、经济学、数学/计算机科学和物理科学)的教授中所占比例不足。在其他科学领域(心理学、生命科学、社会科学),女性的比例要高得多。在这本专著中,我们进行了广泛的生命历程分析,比较了女性和男性在数学密集型领域与非数学密集型领域的同行的轨迹,在非数学密集型领域,女性接近于男性,甚至超过了男性。我们首先研究儿童早期在空间处理方面的差异,然后通过儿童中期和青春期的定量表现来跟踪这一差异,包括高中课程。然后我们将重点放在从高中到大学,再到研究生院,最后到学术科学事业的性别转变上。我们的大量分析结果表明,早期空间和数学推理的性别差异不一定源于生物学基础,女性和男性平均数学能力之间的差距正在缩小(表明强烈的环境影响),右尾数学能力的性别差异随着时间和国籍、种族和其他因素而变化,表明右尾的男女比例可以而且确实会发生变化。我们发现,性别对数学职业和能力的态度和期望(控制实际能力)的差异在幼儿园时就很明显,并且在幼儿园之后有所增加,导致女性在大学时主修数学密集型科目的倾向较低,但女性主修非数学密集型科学的倾向较高,总体而言,科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)专业的女性比例超过50%。大学毕业后,虽然主修数学密集型学科的男性在历史上比女性更常选择并完成这些领域的博士学位,但这一差距最近缩小了三分之二;在非数学密集型的STEM专业中,女性比男性更有可能进入健康和其他与人相关的职业,而不是攻读博士学位。重要的是,在那些在数学密集型领域获得博士学位的人中,进入教授职位的男性和女性有同等的机会获得科学领域的终身学术工作,他们坚持下去,并以相当的比率获得报酬——我们将讨论一些注意事项。从研究生项目到助理教授职位的转变表明,在女性已经非常普遍的领域(心理学、生命科学、社会科学),与女性人数不足但女性担任助理教授职位的人数至少与男性相当(如果不超过男性的话)的数学密集型领域相比,管道泄漏更多。也就是说,数学密集型领域终身职位的面试邀请以及实际的就业机会表明,在数学密集型领域,女性博士申请者的表现至少和男性同行一样好。沿着同样的思路,我们的分析显示,手稿审查和资助是性别中立的:男性和女性作者和主要研究人员的手稿被期刊编辑接受和资助的可能性是一样的,只有极少数例外。在工作时间和每篇出版物的平均引用次数方面,没有明显的性别差异,但在生产率方面,男性总体上有优势。我们试图将这些结果与有关其原因的不同说法相协调,研究了引用、工作时间和兴趣方面的性别差异。我们的结论是,尽管在过去,性别歧视是女性在科学学术生涯中代表性不足的一个重要原因,但在它不再是女性在数学密集型领域代表性不足的一个有效原因后,这一说法仍在继续被援引。因此,目前阻碍女性充分参与数学密集型学术科学领域的障碍源于大学前的因素和随后主修这些领域的可能性,未来的研究应该关注这些障碍,而不是将注意力误导到历史障碍上,这些障碍不再是女性在学术科学中代表性不足的原因。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
68.00
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊介绍: Psychological Science in the Public Interest (PSPI) is a unique journal featuring comprehensive and compelling reviews of issues that are of direct relevance to the general public. These reviews are written by blue ribbon teams of specialists representing a range of viewpoints, and are intended to assess the current state-of-the-science with regard to the topic. Among other things, PSPI reports have challenged the validity of the Rorschach and other projective tests; have explored how to keep the aging brain sharp; and have documented problems with the current state of clinical psychology. PSPI reports are regularly featured in Scientific American Mind and are typically covered in a variety of other major media outlets.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信