Eliminating bias/living with bias?

Jens Chapman
{"title":"Eliminating bias/living with bias?","authors":"Jens Chapman","doi":"10.1055/s-0032-1327802","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Message from the Editor—Eliminating bias/living with bias? The scientific method and elements of bias are fundamentally at odds with one another. While the prior seeks to identify truth through objective analysis, the latter wishes to prove one’s preconceptions through artificially created or filtered observations. Both the scientific method and bias are purpose-driven notions, both serve an intrinsic value. Arguably the more general and truly-lasting societal value is derived from our attempts at approximating truth through scientific methodology, compared with the more selfserving notions of reaffirming our preconceptions through bias. But what if we cannot really separate the two entities—the scientific method and bias? We have explored the theme of bias in previous EBSJ editorials from the perspectives of investigator, commercial, and even publisher bias [1]. We have also learned that characteristics, which favor publication in major scientific medical journals, are United States–based articles that report good outcomes and are commercially funded [2]. Efforts to eliminate bias for the greater good of medicine are still far from complete but the movement to ‘rate’ research based on its Class of Evidence (CoE) is important, as it is basically a rating of the presence of bias. Please look through the Science in Spine article in this EBSJ issue on “Class or level of evidence” by Dettori (page 9–12). Without oversimplifying its content, the entire article underscores the value of a ‘high-class’ study compared with a ‘lower- class evidence’ by enumerating the incessant awareness it takes to eliminate bias with every step of the way of a research project and why the rating of the evidence is so important. Ultimately it is bias-free evidence that reveals the truth behind a hypothesis, shows previously unknown cross-connections, and allows us to rationally formulate new and more effective treatments. The thought of improving upon nature through our medical interventions is perhaps one of the core motivators that has led us into the healthcare field in the first place. As physicians and healthcare providers, we by nature tend to be more optimists, we hope for improvement with our patients, and value positive achievements. However, this disposition may also lead us to displace negative experiences. Perhaps this is a helpful protective phenomenon as it provides us with capacity to move onward. But what if this core proclivity toward optimism is a problem in itself by introducing bias not only in our clinical medicine practices but also in our approach toward research? This unsettling thought was raised by Emerson et al (full disclosure: some of the authors are fellow employees at the University of Washington) in their recent article titled “Testing for a positive outcome bias in peer review: a randomized controlled trial.” With permission from the editors-inchief of two leading orthopaedic publications, JBJS and CORR, the authors submitted a fabricated sham article either with positive outcomes or the very same sham article with ‘no-difference’ findings to 210 regular reviewers. A statistically significant number of reviewers recommended publication of the positive outcomes version of the sham paper compared with the negative version (97.3% versus 80.0%, P < .005) [3].","PeriodicalId":89675,"journal":{"name":"Evidence-based spine-care journal","volume":"3 3","pages":"5-6"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2012-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1055/s-0032-1327802","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Evidence-based spine-care journal","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1327802","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Message from the Editor—Eliminating bias/living with bias? The scientific method and elements of bias are fundamentally at odds with one another. While the prior seeks to identify truth through objective analysis, the latter wishes to prove one’s preconceptions through artificially created or filtered observations. Both the scientific method and bias are purpose-driven notions, both serve an intrinsic value. Arguably the more general and truly-lasting societal value is derived from our attempts at approximating truth through scientific methodology, compared with the more selfserving notions of reaffirming our preconceptions through bias. But what if we cannot really separate the two entities—the scientific method and bias? We have explored the theme of bias in previous EBSJ editorials from the perspectives of investigator, commercial, and even publisher bias [1]. We have also learned that characteristics, which favor publication in major scientific medical journals, are United States–based articles that report good outcomes and are commercially funded [2]. Efforts to eliminate bias for the greater good of medicine are still far from complete but the movement to ‘rate’ research based on its Class of Evidence (CoE) is important, as it is basically a rating of the presence of bias. Please look through the Science in Spine article in this EBSJ issue on “Class or level of evidence” by Dettori (page 9–12). Without oversimplifying its content, the entire article underscores the value of a ‘high-class’ study compared with a ‘lower- class evidence’ by enumerating the incessant awareness it takes to eliminate bias with every step of the way of a research project and why the rating of the evidence is so important. Ultimately it is bias-free evidence that reveals the truth behind a hypothesis, shows previously unknown cross-connections, and allows us to rationally formulate new and more effective treatments. The thought of improving upon nature through our medical interventions is perhaps one of the core motivators that has led us into the healthcare field in the first place. As physicians and healthcare providers, we by nature tend to be more optimists, we hope for improvement with our patients, and value positive achievements. However, this disposition may also lead us to displace negative experiences. Perhaps this is a helpful protective phenomenon as it provides us with capacity to move onward. But what if this core proclivity toward optimism is a problem in itself by introducing bias not only in our clinical medicine practices but also in our approach toward research? This unsettling thought was raised by Emerson et al (full disclosure: some of the authors are fellow employees at the University of Washington) in their recent article titled “Testing for a positive outcome bias in peer review: a randomized controlled trial.” With permission from the editors-inchief of two leading orthopaedic publications, JBJS and CORR, the authors submitted a fabricated sham article either with positive outcomes or the very same sham article with ‘no-difference’ findings to 210 regular reviewers. A statistically significant number of reviewers recommended publication of the positive outcomes version of the sham paper compared with the negative version (97.3% versus 80.0%, P < .005) [3].

Abstract Image

Abstract Image

消除偏见/带着偏见生活?
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信