Commentary on Witnessing Abusive Head Trauma: Accidents Show Higher Rates of Intracranial Pathologies Than Shaking—Caution Is Warranted

Christopher Greeley, Jim Anderst
{"title":"Commentary on Witnessing Abusive Head Trauma: Accidents Show Higher Rates of Intracranial Pathologies Than Shaking—Caution Is Warranted","authors":"Christopher Greeley,&nbsp;Jim Anderst","doi":"10.1002/cns3.70030","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>As the topic of abusive head trauma (AHT) has become a cause célèbre in some nonpediatric academic circles, we read the research article by Brook [<span>1</span>] with great interest. The research article reports on an effort to compare some of the findings typically associated with AHT between cases in which shaking was witnessed or admitted to those in which accidents were confirmed. Initially, it appears to be similar to prior studies comparing witnessed accidents with witnessed/admitted abuse [<span>2, 3</span>] but particularly focused on shaking as a reported mechanism. The author takes a unique approach in attempting to answer the question about the rate of findings between accidental and inflicted head injuries due to shaking. The author's chosen methodology contains several flaws, which are quite puzzling. We chose to specifically highlight two fundamental flaws that we feel undermine meaningful conclusions: the study designs of the chosen data sets and the populations being compared between the categories of cases.</p><p>The first concern is the data sets that were constructed. The author used four independent sources from which he has grouped participants into three categories: unconflicted witnessed accident, witnessed shaking, and unconflicted witnessed shaking. The first data set assembled (unconflicted witnessed accident) was of participants extracted from the PediBIRN study (www.pedibirn.com) and as reported in Hymel et al. [<span>4</span>]. PediBIRN is a prospective, cohort study of children younger than 3 years admitted to pediatric intensive care units for traumatic head injury. Children who are injured in accidents and admitted to an intensive care unit represent the most severe accidents only. The second data set assembled (unconflicted witnessed shaking) was from the Swedish National Patient Register and reported by Thiblin et al. [<span>5</span>]. This is a retrospective case series of infant victims of abuse identified by ICD-10 codes. In this data set, shaking is not an explicit variable, and the author only includes those participants in which shaking was expressly reported in the medical records. Records that did not include a reference to shaking (i.e., missing data) were treated the same as those in which shaking was specifically denied (i.e., shaking = “no”), likely resulting in undercounting of shaking. The author apparently includes the same 36 participants identified by Thiblin et al. [<span>5</span>]. If so, it is important to note that Thiblin et al. [<span>5</span>] has since received an “Expression of Concern” (similar to a “black box warning”) by the editors of <i>PLoS One</i>, in part due to “suitability of the study design to support the conclusions” [<span>6</span>]. The last category assembled (witnessed shaking) was from two separate sources, both reported in Feldman et al. [<span>7</span>]. The ExSTRA study was a prospective, multicenter, observational study of children younger than 10 years referred for evaluation for suspected physical abuse, from which Brook sampled 17 participants with witnessed shaking. The second was “Helfer Society data,” from which the author sampled 6 participants from a haphazard sample obtained from a professional society listserv, identified by a reported history of shaking without impact. The three groups for comparison (unconflicted witnessed accident, witnessed shaking, and unconflicted witnessed shaking) were constructed from four different data sources. This is the first fatal flaw: combining participants from studies with different methodologies, that have been assembled by different criteria, from populations with different base rates, and treating them as if the same.</p><p>This study approach violates two tenets of clinical research. First, the directionality of case-control or cohort studies cannot be reversed, and second, rates cannot be compared across study designs [<span>8</span>]. It is important to emphasize that (a) within a particular study design, the direction of association is <i>not</i> reversible, and (b) rates of either exposures or outcomes are <i>not</i> comparable across different study designs. In this study, the “unconflicted witnessed accident” group [<span>4</span>] was assembled from a prospective cohort study of children in intensive care units. The “witnessed shaking” group [<span>7</span>] was a combination of a prospective, multicenter, observational study and a haphazard series from a professional listserv. The “unconflicted witnessed shaking” group [<span>5</span>] was assembled from a retrospective case series. The full data set represents a blending of prospective and retrospective, case series, case–control and cohort series, and haphazard sampling. This mélange of study designs cannot simply be placed side by side and compared. While these different study methodologies can be complementary, their data cannot be simply combined or compared.</p><p>The second concerning methodological flaw is related to the populations reported in each of the data sets. As noted, the four data sets represent different study designs (with different inclusion and exclusion criteria) and sample distinct populations. PediBIRN represented children under 3 years, admitted to pediatric intensive care units with suspected traumatic head trauma. It is important to note that accidentally injured children were “preselected” for increased severity, as the vast majority of children injured in accidents are not admitted to an intensive care unit. ExSTRA was a group of children under 10 years of age being evaluated for suspected physical abuse. Many of these children were seen in outpatient settings. Thiblin et al.'s [<span>5</span>] study included infants on a national register for various forms of maltreatment over 20 years, presumably across different care settings. Lastly, the Helfer Society was a nonsequential, haphazard series of undefined age or time span. The rates of either exposures or outcomes are not comparable given the clearly different populations. To illustrate the importance of representative populations, combining the four data sets as done in the current study would be akin to a study on lung cancer and combining data on adult cigarette smokers, teen nonsmokers, toddlers, and retired coal miners.</p><p>Given the concerns noted earlier, we caution the reader not to draw meaningful conclusions based on this study. While the author does mention “not uniform” data sets within the Limitations section of the manuscript, we believe that this minimizes the significant implications of different data, variables, study designs, and analyses. The author notes that his findings are consistent with Thiblin et al. [<span>9</span>], a study that suffers from similar methodological shortcomings [<span>10</span>]. The author's conclusions that intracranial findings typically associated with AHT are more common in accidental injuries as compared with reported shaking injuries are simply unsupported by the methodology utilized.</p><p><b>Christopher Greeley:</b> conceptualization, methodology, writing – review and editing, writing – original draft. <b>Jim Anderst:</b> conceptualization, writing – original draft, methodology, writing – review and editing.</p><p>The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p>","PeriodicalId":72232,"journal":{"name":"Annals of the Child Neurology Society","volume":"3 3","pages":"253-254"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2025-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/cns3.70030","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Annals of the Child Neurology Society","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cns3.70030","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

As the topic of abusive head trauma (AHT) has become a cause célèbre in some nonpediatric academic circles, we read the research article by Brook [1] with great interest. The research article reports on an effort to compare some of the findings typically associated with AHT between cases in which shaking was witnessed or admitted to those in which accidents were confirmed. Initially, it appears to be similar to prior studies comparing witnessed accidents with witnessed/admitted abuse [2, 3] but particularly focused on shaking as a reported mechanism. The author takes a unique approach in attempting to answer the question about the rate of findings between accidental and inflicted head injuries due to shaking. The author's chosen methodology contains several flaws, which are quite puzzling. We chose to specifically highlight two fundamental flaws that we feel undermine meaningful conclusions: the study designs of the chosen data sets and the populations being compared between the categories of cases.

The first concern is the data sets that were constructed. The author used four independent sources from which he has grouped participants into three categories: unconflicted witnessed accident, witnessed shaking, and unconflicted witnessed shaking. The first data set assembled (unconflicted witnessed accident) was of participants extracted from the PediBIRN study (www.pedibirn.com) and as reported in Hymel et al. [4]. PediBIRN is a prospective, cohort study of children younger than 3 years admitted to pediatric intensive care units for traumatic head injury. Children who are injured in accidents and admitted to an intensive care unit represent the most severe accidents only. The second data set assembled (unconflicted witnessed shaking) was from the Swedish National Patient Register and reported by Thiblin et al. [5]. This is a retrospective case series of infant victims of abuse identified by ICD-10 codes. In this data set, shaking is not an explicit variable, and the author only includes those participants in which shaking was expressly reported in the medical records. Records that did not include a reference to shaking (i.e., missing data) were treated the same as those in which shaking was specifically denied (i.e., shaking = “no”), likely resulting in undercounting of shaking. The author apparently includes the same 36 participants identified by Thiblin et al. [5]. If so, it is important to note that Thiblin et al. [5] has since received an “Expression of Concern” (similar to a “black box warning”) by the editors of PLoS One, in part due to “suitability of the study design to support the conclusions” [6]. The last category assembled (witnessed shaking) was from two separate sources, both reported in Feldman et al. [7]. The ExSTRA study was a prospective, multicenter, observational study of children younger than 10 years referred for evaluation for suspected physical abuse, from which Brook sampled 17 participants with witnessed shaking. The second was “Helfer Society data,” from which the author sampled 6 participants from a haphazard sample obtained from a professional society listserv, identified by a reported history of shaking without impact. The three groups for comparison (unconflicted witnessed accident, witnessed shaking, and unconflicted witnessed shaking) were constructed from four different data sources. This is the first fatal flaw: combining participants from studies with different methodologies, that have been assembled by different criteria, from populations with different base rates, and treating them as if the same.

This study approach violates two tenets of clinical research. First, the directionality of case-control or cohort studies cannot be reversed, and second, rates cannot be compared across study designs [8]. It is important to emphasize that (a) within a particular study design, the direction of association is not reversible, and (b) rates of either exposures or outcomes are not comparable across different study designs. In this study, the “unconflicted witnessed accident” group [4] was assembled from a prospective cohort study of children in intensive care units. The “witnessed shaking” group [7] was a combination of a prospective, multicenter, observational study and a haphazard series from a professional listserv. The “unconflicted witnessed shaking” group [5] was assembled from a retrospective case series. The full data set represents a blending of prospective and retrospective, case series, case–control and cohort series, and haphazard sampling. This mélange of study designs cannot simply be placed side by side and compared. While these different study methodologies can be complementary, their data cannot be simply combined or compared.

The second concerning methodological flaw is related to the populations reported in each of the data sets. As noted, the four data sets represent different study designs (with different inclusion and exclusion criteria) and sample distinct populations. PediBIRN represented children under 3 years, admitted to pediatric intensive care units with suspected traumatic head trauma. It is important to note that accidentally injured children were “preselected” for increased severity, as the vast majority of children injured in accidents are not admitted to an intensive care unit. ExSTRA was a group of children under 10 years of age being evaluated for suspected physical abuse. Many of these children were seen in outpatient settings. Thiblin et al.'s [5] study included infants on a national register for various forms of maltreatment over 20 years, presumably across different care settings. Lastly, the Helfer Society was a nonsequential, haphazard series of undefined age or time span. The rates of either exposures or outcomes are not comparable given the clearly different populations. To illustrate the importance of representative populations, combining the four data sets as done in the current study would be akin to a study on lung cancer and combining data on adult cigarette smokers, teen nonsmokers, toddlers, and retired coal miners.

Given the concerns noted earlier, we caution the reader not to draw meaningful conclusions based on this study. While the author does mention “not uniform” data sets within the Limitations section of the manuscript, we believe that this minimizes the significant implications of different data, variables, study designs, and analyses. The author notes that his findings are consistent with Thiblin et al. [9], a study that suffers from similar methodological shortcomings [10]. The author's conclusions that intracranial findings typically associated with AHT are more common in accidental injuries as compared with reported shaking injuries are simply unsupported by the methodology utilized.

Christopher Greeley: conceptualization, methodology, writing – review and editing, writing – original draft. Jim Anderst: conceptualization, writing – original draft, methodology, writing – review and editing.

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

目睹虐待性头部创伤的评论:事故显示颅内病变的发生率高于震动-谨慎是有必要的
由于虐待性头部创伤(AHT)的话题已经成为一些非儿科学术界的一个争议话题,我们怀着极大的兴趣阅读了Brook[1]的研究文章。这篇研究文章报道了一项比较AHT的一些典型发现的努力,这些发现是在目击或承认发生摇晃的病例与确认发生事故的病例之间进行的。最初,它似乎与先前的研究相似,将目击事故与目击/承认的虐待进行比较[2,3],但特别关注摇晃作为报道的机制。作者采取了一种独特的方法,试图回答关于意外和造成的头部伤害之间的调查结果率的问题,由于摇晃。作者选择的方法有几个缺陷,这是相当令人费解的。我们选择特别强调两个我们认为会破坏有意义结论的基本缺陷:所选数据集的研究设计和在病例类别之间进行比较的总体。第一个问题是所构造的数据集。作者使用了四个独立的来源,他将参与者分为三类:无冲突的目击事故,目击摇晃和无冲突的目击摇晃。收集的第一个数据集(无冲突的目击事故)是从PediBIRN研究(www.pedibirn.com)中提取的参与者,Hymel等人报道过。PediBIRN是一项前瞻性队列研究,研究对象是因颅脑外伤入住儿科重症监护病房的3岁以下儿童。在事故中受伤并住进重症监护病房的儿童只代表最严重的事故。收集的第二组数据集(无冲突的目击震动)来自瑞典国家患者登记册,由Thiblin等人报道。这是根据ICD-10代码确定的虐待婴儿受害者的回顾性病例系列。在这个数据集中,摇晃不是一个显式变量,作者只包括那些在医疗记录中明确报告摇晃的参与者。不包括震动参考的记录(即,缺失的数据)被视为与那些明确否认震动的记录(即,震动=“否”)相同,可能导致震动的少计。作者显然包括了Thiblin等人所确定的36名参与者。如果是这样,值得注意的是,Thiblin等人已经收到了PLoS One编辑的“关注表达”(类似于“黑框警告”),部分原因是“研究设计是否适合支持结论”[6]。最后一类(亲眼目睹的震动)来自两个不同的来源,均由Feldman等人报道。ExSTRA研究是一项前瞻性的、多中心的观察性研究,研究对象是10岁以下的儿童,他们因涉嫌身体虐待而接受评估,布鲁克从中抽取了17名目击摇晃的参与者。第二个是“Helfer Society数据”,作者从一个专业协会的名单中随机抽取了6名参与者,根据报告的震动历史来确定没有影响。从四个不同的数据源构建了三个比较组(无冲突目击事故、目击震动和无冲突目击震动)。这是第一个致命的缺陷:将采用不同方法、按不同标准、从不同基本比率的人群中收集的研究对象组合在一起,并将他们当作相同的对象对待。这种研究方法违反了临床研究的两个原则。首先,病例对照或队列研究的方向性不能逆转,其次,不同研究设计之间的比率不能进行比较[10]。需要强调的是:(a)在一个特定的研究设计中,关联的方向是不可逆转的,(b)在不同的研究设计中,暴露率或结果率是不可比较的。在本研究中,“无冲突目击事故”组[4]是从重症监护病房儿童的前瞻性队列研究中聚集而来的。“目击震动”组[7]是一项前瞻性、多中心、观察性研究和来自专业列表服务的随机系列研究的结合。“无冲突目击震动”组[5]是由回顾性病例系列组成的。完整的数据集混合了前瞻性和回顾性、病例系列、病例对照和队列系列以及随机抽样。这一系列的研究设计不能简单地放在一起进行比较。虽然这些不同的研究方法可以互补,但它们的数据不能简单地组合或比较。第二个关于方法上的缺陷与每个数据集中报告的人口有关。 如上所述,这四个数据集代表了不同的研究设计(具有不同的纳入和排除标准)和不同的样本人群。PediBIRN代表3岁以下儿童,因疑似颅脑外伤而入住儿科重症监护病房。值得注意的是,意外受伤的儿童被“预选”为严重程度增加,因为绝大多数在事故中受伤的儿童不被送进重症监护病房。ExSTRA是一群10岁以下的儿童,因涉嫌身体虐待而接受评估。这些儿童中有许多是在门诊就诊的。Thiblin等人的[5]研究包括了20多年来在国家登记的各种形式的虐待的婴儿,可能是在不同的护理环境中。最后,Helfer Society是一个无顺序的、随意的、年龄或时间跨度未确定的系列。鉴于明显不同的人群,暴露率或结果都不可比较。为了说明代表性人群的重要性,将当前研究中所做的四个数据集结合起来,就像对肺癌的研究一样,将成年吸烟者、青少年不吸烟者、幼儿和退休煤矿工人的数据结合起来。考虑到前面提到的问题,我们提醒读者不要根据这项研究得出有意义的结论。虽然作者在文章的局限性部分提到了“不统一”的数据集,但我们认为这将不同数据、变量、研究设计和分析的重要含义降到最低。作者指出,他的发现与Thiblin等人的研究结果一致,后者的研究也存在类似的方法缺陷。作者的结论是,与报道的震动损伤相比,与AHT相关的颅内发现在意外伤害中更常见,这一结论根本不支持所采用的方法。克里斯托弗·格里利:概念,方法论,写作-审查和编辑,写作-原稿。吉姆·安德斯特:概念化,写作-原稿,方法论,写作-审查和编辑。作者声明无利益冲突。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信