Patrick T. Donahue, Jennifer A. Schrack, Johannes Thrul, Michelle C. Carlson
{"title":"Response to: Refining the clinical interpretation of activity variability in cognitive impairment: The need for phenotypic specificity","authors":"Patrick T. Donahue, Jennifer A. Schrack, Johannes Thrul, Michelle C. Carlson","doi":"10.1002/trc2.70128","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dear Guo et al.,</p><p>We thank Guo et al.<span><sup>1</sup></span> for their interest in our article<span><sup>2</sup></span> and for providing innovative thoughts regarding clinical translation of our activity variability metric.</p><p>We would first like to emphasize that we positioned our work as an exploratory analysis of a conceptually novel metric developed using available accelerometry data. It is fit for application in future longitudinal and clinical studies with accelerometry data, which can replicate our findings and establish clinically meaningful changes relevant to activity variability and cognition. Our cross-sectional study suggests that low activity variability is strongly associated with cognitive impairment, yet we acknowledge that this metric requires further validation before implementation as a digital biomarker of cognitive decline and impairment. We intend to extend this work to other studies and encourage other researchers and clinicians to pursue validation studies, which were beyond the scope of our analysis.</p><p>We agree with Guo et al.<span><sup>1</sup></span> that cognitive impairment is heterogeneous and that it would be useful to distinguish subtypes of dementia, such as vascular versus Alzheimer's disease pathologies. The data we used have strengths and limitations, as noted in our article. As Guo et al. mention, and we discussed in our article, the National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) dementia classification criteria are <i>not</i> equivalent to a dementia diagnosis, nor do they distinguish subtypes of cognitive decline and impairment.<span><sup>2</sup></span> As Guo et al. compellingly state, it is important to further extend the conceptual link between activity variability and cognitive risk to clinical neurological outcomes, not limited to dementia (e.g., traumatic brain injury). Guo et al.’s proposal to link activity variability to specific brain regions via neuroimaging and associated cognitive domains via neuropsychological testing presents exciting additional mechanistic areas of research, which would greatly complement our findings. We agree that identifying underlying brain regions and cognitive domains that may be specifically related to activity variability would enhance this metric's clinical utility for greater sensitivity in detection of cognitive and functional impairment. In addition, if activity variability can be linked to specific brain regions, it may serve as a potential intermediate outcome for cognitive interventions.</p><p>Guo et al. commented that, because activity variability and gait speed were correlated, we cannot disentangle “the cognitive versus biomechanical determinants of variability”. Importantly, our study was not intended to examine the cognitive versus biological determinants of activity variability. Although activity variability and gait speed were correlated, activity variability remained strongly associated with cognitive impairment even after controlling for gait speed in our analysis (Model 4 in Table 2), suggesting that activity variability is independently associated with cognitive impairment. Moreover, we do not advocate that activity variability reflects a specific neurological or causal pathway. Section 4.3 of our article (“Possible mechanisms”) discusses potential hypotheses not specified a priori, which may explain the observed associations in our article. The goal of our work was to examine whether activity variability may be a non-invasive and valuable functional marker of cognitive impairment. As noted above, important future work by our team and others will need to apply this metric to longitudinal data to examine causal pathways and potential mediating mechanisms, which were beyond the scope of our cross-sectional analysis.</p><p>The multidimensional framework that Guo et al. mentioned appears interesting and posits a composition of several risk factors for cognitive decline, including activity variability. However, it is unclear whether they are proposing a biological framework for cognitive impairment, or are interested in designing a prediction model to identify patients at high risk for cognitive impairment. Either of these objectives may be useful, and we encourage Guo et al. and others to consider assessing activity variability in other cohorts with accelerometry data. In future work, it will be important to include detailed neuroimaging, biological risk factors (e.g., amyloid, tau, apolipoprotein E), and longitudinal follow-up for clinically adjudicated dementia outcomes. We hope that other researchers and clinicians see the promise of digital biomarkers–like activity variability–and will consider examining these biomarkers as informative metrics in dementia prevention, treatment, and care.</p><p>Jennifer A. Schrack is a consultant for Edwards Lifesciences. Michelle C. Carlson serves as a scientific advisory board member of AARP Staying Sharp. Patrick T. Donahue and Johannes Thru have no conflicts to disclose. Author disclosures are available in the supporting information.</p>","PeriodicalId":53225,"journal":{"name":"Alzheimer''s and Dementia: Translational Research and Clinical Interventions","volume":"11 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.9000,"publicationDate":"2025-06-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/trc2.70128","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Alzheimer''s and Dementia: Translational Research and Clinical Interventions","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/trc2.70128","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"CLINICAL NEUROLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
Dear Guo et al.,
We thank Guo et al.1 for their interest in our article2 and for providing innovative thoughts regarding clinical translation of our activity variability metric.
We would first like to emphasize that we positioned our work as an exploratory analysis of a conceptually novel metric developed using available accelerometry data. It is fit for application in future longitudinal and clinical studies with accelerometry data, which can replicate our findings and establish clinically meaningful changes relevant to activity variability and cognition. Our cross-sectional study suggests that low activity variability is strongly associated with cognitive impairment, yet we acknowledge that this metric requires further validation before implementation as a digital biomarker of cognitive decline and impairment. We intend to extend this work to other studies and encourage other researchers and clinicians to pursue validation studies, which were beyond the scope of our analysis.
We agree with Guo et al.1 that cognitive impairment is heterogeneous and that it would be useful to distinguish subtypes of dementia, such as vascular versus Alzheimer's disease pathologies. The data we used have strengths and limitations, as noted in our article. As Guo et al. mention, and we discussed in our article, the National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) dementia classification criteria are not equivalent to a dementia diagnosis, nor do they distinguish subtypes of cognitive decline and impairment.2 As Guo et al. compellingly state, it is important to further extend the conceptual link between activity variability and cognitive risk to clinical neurological outcomes, not limited to dementia (e.g., traumatic brain injury). Guo et al.’s proposal to link activity variability to specific brain regions via neuroimaging and associated cognitive domains via neuropsychological testing presents exciting additional mechanistic areas of research, which would greatly complement our findings. We agree that identifying underlying brain regions and cognitive domains that may be specifically related to activity variability would enhance this metric's clinical utility for greater sensitivity in detection of cognitive and functional impairment. In addition, if activity variability can be linked to specific brain regions, it may serve as a potential intermediate outcome for cognitive interventions.
Guo et al. commented that, because activity variability and gait speed were correlated, we cannot disentangle “the cognitive versus biomechanical determinants of variability”. Importantly, our study was not intended to examine the cognitive versus biological determinants of activity variability. Although activity variability and gait speed were correlated, activity variability remained strongly associated with cognitive impairment even after controlling for gait speed in our analysis (Model 4 in Table 2), suggesting that activity variability is independently associated with cognitive impairment. Moreover, we do not advocate that activity variability reflects a specific neurological or causal pathway. Section 4.3 of our article (“Possible mechanisms”) discusses potential hypotheses not specified a priori, which may explain the observed associations in our article. The goal of our work was to examine whether activity variability may be a non-invasive and valuable functional marker of cognitive impairment. As noted above, important future work by our team and others will need to apply this metric to longitudinal data to examine causal pathways and potential mediating mechanisms, which were beyond the scope of our cross-sectional analysis.
The multidimensional framework that Guo et al. mentioned appears interesting and posits a composition of several risk factors for cognitive decline, including activity variability. However, it is unclear whether they are proposing a biological framework for cognitive impairment, or are interested in designing a prediction model to identify patients at high risk for cognitive impairment. Either of these objectives may be useful, and we encourage Guo et al. and others to consider assessing activity variability in other cohorts with accelerometry data. In future work, it will be important to include detailed neuroimaging, biological risk factors (e.g., amyloid, tau, apolipoprotein E), and longitudinal follow-up for clinically adjudicated dementia outcomes. We hope that other researchers and clinicians see the promise of digital biomarkers–like activity variability–and will consider examining these biomarkers as informative metrics in dementia prevention, treatment, and care.
Jennifer A. Schrack is a consultant for Edwards Lifesciences. Michelle C. Carlson serves as a scientific advisory board member of AARP Staying Sharp. Patrick T. Donahue and Johannes Thru have no conflicts to disclose. Author disclosures are available in the supporting information.
亲爱的郭等人:我们感谢郭等人1对我们的文章2的兴趣,并为我们的活动可变性指标的临床翻译提供了创新的想法。我们首先要强调的是,我们将我们的工作定位为利用可用的加速度测量数据开发的概念新颖度量的探索性分析。它适合应用于未来的纵向和临床研究的加速度测量数据,可以重复我们的发现,并建立有关活动变异性和认知的临床有意义的变化。我们的横断面研究表明,低活动变异性与认知障碍密切相关,但我们承认,在作为认知衰退和障碍的数字生物标志物实施之前,这一指标需要进一步验证。我们打算将这项工作扩展到其他研究,并鼓励其他研究人员和临床医生进行超出我们分析范围的验证研究。我们同意Guo等人1的观点,即认知障碍是异质性的,区分痴呆的亚型(如血管与阿尔茨海默病病理)将是有用的。正如我们在文章中提到的,我们使用的数据有优点和局限性。正如郭等人提到的,以及我们在文章中讨论的,国家健康与老龄化趋势研究(NHATS)痴呆分类标准并不等同于痴呆诊断,也不能区分认知能力下降和损伤的亚型正如Guo等人令人信服地指出的那样,重要的是进一步将活动变异性和认知风险之间的概念联系扩展到临床神经学结果,而不仅仅局限于痴呆(例如,创伤性脑损伤)。Guo等人通过神经成像将活动变异性与特定的大脑区域联系起来,并通过神经心理学测试将相关的认知领域联系起来,这一建议提出了令人兴奋的额外机制研究领域,这将极大地补充我们的发现。我们同意,识别可能与活动变异性特别相关的潜在大脑区域和认知域将提高该指标在检测认知和功能障碍方面的临床实用性。此外,如果活动可变性可以与特定的大脑区域联系起来,它可能作为认知干预的潜在中间结果。Guo等人评论说,由于活动可变性和步态速度是相关的,我们无法区分“可变性的认知因素和生物力学因素”。重要的是,我们的研究并不是为了检查活动可变性的认知和生物决定因素。尽管活动变异性和步态速度相关,但在我们的分析中,即使控制了步态速度(表2中的模型4),活动变异性仍然与认知障碍密切相关,这表明活动变异性与认知障碍独立相关。此外,我们不主张活动可变性反映了特定的神经或因果途径。我们文章的第4.3节(“可能的机制”)讨论了未指定先验的潜在假设,这可以解释我们文章中观察到的关联。我们工作的目的是检查活动变异性是否可能是认知障碍的非侵入性和有价值的功能标记。如上所述,我们团队和其他人未来的重要工作将需要将这一指标应用于纵向数据,以检查因果途径和潜在的中介机制,这超出了我们横断面分析的范围。Guo等人提到的多维框架看起来很有趣,它假设了认知能力下降的几个风险因素的组成,包括活动的可变性。然而,目前尚不清楚他们是在提出认知障碍的生物学框架,还是有兴趣设计一种预测模型来识别认知障碍高风险患者。这些目标中的任何一个都可能有用,我们鼓励Guo等人考虑使用加速度计数据评估其他队列的活动变异性。在未来的工作中,包括详细的神经影像学、生物学危险因素(如淀粉样蛋白、tau蛋白、载脂蛋白E)和临床判定痴呆结局的纵向随访将是很重要的。我们希望其他研究人员和临床医生看到数字生物标志物的前景,比如活动变异性,并考虑将这些生物标志物作为痴呆症预防、治疗和护理的信息指标。Jennifer a . Schrack是Edwards生命科学公司的顾问。Michelle C. Carlson是AARP Staying Sharp的科学顾问委员会成员。帕特里克·t·多纳休和约翰内斯·瑟鲁没有要透露的冲突。作者披露可在支持信息中获得。
期刊介绍:
Alzheimer''s & Dementia: Translational Research & Clinical Interventions (TRCI) is a peer-reviewed, open access,journal from the Alzheimer''s Association®. The journal seeks to bridge the full scope of explorations between basic research on drug discovery and clinical studies, validating putative therapies for aging-related chronic brain conditions that affect cognition, motor functions, and other behavioral or clinical symptoms associated with all forms dementia and Alzheimer''s disease. The journal will publish findings from diverse domains of research and disciplines to accelerate the conversion of abstract facts into practical knowledge: specifically, to translate what is learned at the bench into bedside applications. The journal seeks to publish articles that go beyond a singular emphasis on either basic drug discovery research or clinical research. Rather, an important theme of articles will be the linkages between and among the various discrete steps in the complex continuum of therapy development. For rapid communication among a multidisciplinary research audience involving the range of therapeutic interventions, TRCI will consider only original contributions that include feature length research articles, systematic reviews, meta-analyses, brief reports, narrative reviews, commentaries, letters, perspectives, and research news that would advance wide range of interventions to ameliorate symptoms or alter the progression of chronic neurocognitive disorders such as dementia and Alzheimer''s disease. The journal will publish on topics related to medicine, geriatrics, neuroscience, neurophysiology, neurology, psychiatry, clinical psychology, bioinformatics, pharmaco-genetics, regulatory issues, health economics, pharmacoeconomics, and public health policy as these apply to preclinical and clinical research on therapeutics.