{"title":"In the Land of the Blind: The Statistical Competency Paradox in Ecological Research","authors":"Rafael Dettogni Guariento","doi":"10.1002/bes2.2230","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>“In the land of the blind, the one-eyed man is king”—this old Dutch proverb carries a deep message about relative advantage and perspective. At its core, the saying suggests that a person with limited knowledge becomes an expert among those who know nothing about the subject or that mediocre talents shine brightly when surrounded by complete beginners. The wisdom of this saying reminds us that value is often relative rather than absolute. It encourages us to recognize that what might seem like a modest capability in one context could be extraordinarily valuable in another. However, there is also a subtle warning within this proverb: what appears to be exceptional in one environment might actually be quite limited in the broader world. The one-eyed person is only “king” in the land of the blind; in a land of people with full vision, they would not hold the same advantage.</p><p>This old proverb finds a fascinating parallel in the world of ecological research, particularly regarding statistical analyses and methodological debates during the peer review process. As the editor of an ecology journal myself, I frequently encounter peculiar situations that perfectly illustrate this proverb. I regularly witness intense battles between reviewers and authors over statistical analyses. These methodological debates often become surprisingly heated, with reviewers demanding specific statistical approaches or criticizing authors' analytical choices with remarkable confidence. The authors, in turn, defend their methods with equal vigor or propose alternative analyses with similar conviction. I am an author of scientific papers too and I have been in this battlefield before. But what makes this scenario particularly fascinating is my privileged position as an editor. I can see the academic backgrounds of both the reviewers (who are not anonymous to editors) and the authors, revealing that these passionate statistical arguments are typically occurring between researchers whose primary expertise lies in other areas. For instance, heated debates about the appropriateness of certain probability distributions, statistical packages for specific analyses, the interpretation of main and interactive effects, or the application of mixed models often occur without an adequate understanding of the underlying mathematical principles (Ellison and Dennis <span>2010</span>). This creates a rather intriguing situation in scientific publishing in ecology: Complex statistical discussions and decisions about methodological rigor are being arbitrated by researchers who, while potentially excellent in their primary field of study, often lack the formal mathematical or statistical training that would traditionally qualify one to make such judgments.</p><p>This situation perfectly embodies the proverb's wisdom. Ecologists who have taken a few additional statistics courses or who have spent more time self-studying mathematical concepts often find themselves in positions of significant influence when reviewing or critiquing others' work. Their relatively modest mathematical background, which might seem basic to a trained statistician, becomes a powerful advantage in a field where many researchers have even less statistical training. This dynamic creates what we might term the “statistical competency paradox” in ecological research: While these discussions are crucial for advancing ecological research, they are often conducted by researchers whose statistical expertise is relatively limited compared to actual statisticians (see Chapter 11 in Taper and Lele <span>2010</span>).</p><p>Rather than viewing this situation as a critique of ecological research, we could also see it as an opportunity for growth and improvement. It highlights a unique characteristic of the field: Ecologists must grapple with incredibly complex systems using mathematical tools they were not primarily trained to use. And this challenge is not unique to statistics (Fox <span>2013</span>). The “one-eyed kings” in these scenarios (those with somewhat more statistical knowledge) may play an important role in raising the bar for methodological rigor, even if their own expertise is limited compared to professional statisticians. While modest statistical proficiency may not suffice for conducting complex analyses, it empowers researchers to make informed methodological choices and collaborate effectively with experts when needed. Thus, on the other hand, even a foundational understanding of statistical principles can hold immense value in ecological research. Researchers with basic statistical knowledge are better equipped to design experiments, interpret results, and critically assess the methodologies employed by others. This competency helps mitigate common errors, such as misinterpretation of statistical tests or misuse of analytical tools, thereby improving the overall reliability of ecological findings. Moreover, it fosters a culture of statistical literacy within the ecological community, bridging the gap between advanced statistical techniques and practical ecological applications. In this sense, even modest statistical knowledge may serve as a cornerstone for robust and impactful ecological research (Barraquand et al. <span>2014</span>).</p><p>However, in the often contentious realm of methodological debates, it is easy to overestimate our expertise (Steel et al. <span>2013</span>). Researchers, driven by the desire to defend or critique statistical methods, may overlook the limits of their own statistical training. This overconfidence can stifle constructive dialogue and lead to misinformed conclusions. Acknowledging our limitations is not a sign of weakness but a hallmark of scientific integrity. By embracing intellectual humility, researchers create space for meaningful exchanges of ideas and foster a culture of continuous learning. Recognizing gaps in our knowledge encourages us to seek input from those with specialized expertise, ultimately strengthening the rigor of scientific discourse. In this way, humility not only improves the quality of individual contributions but also enhances the collective advancement of science.</p><p>To address the statistical competency paradox, it is essential to enhance statistical training in ecological education. This could involve incorporating more advanced statistical courses into ecology programs, encouraging students to take courses in statistics or biostatistics, and providing opportunities for hands-on statistical training (Touchon and McCoy <span>2016</span>). By equipping ecologists with a stronger foundation in statistics, we can improve the overall quality of ecological research. To meet this demand, academic programs should integrate advanced statistical methods and data analysis courses into core curricula. This integration should be accompanied by hands-on workshops and projects that involve real-world ecological datasets, encouraging students to apply and refine statistical skills in realistic contexts. Additionally, programs should encourage students to take courses in statistics, biostatistics, or data science offered by mathematics or computer science departments, fostering a multidisciplinary perspective. Such initiatives would reduce overreliance on “one-eyed kings” (researchers with limited statistical expertise perceived as experts in their peer groups) and democratize access to statistical knowledge. Moreover, equipping ecologists with advanced statistical tools empowers them to conduct high-quality, independent analyses and critically evaluate the work of others, strengthening the overall research ecosystem.</p><p>To bridge gaps in expertise, it is crucial to cultivate stronger collaborations between ecologists and professional statisticians. These partnerships could take several forms, including coauthorship and joint projects where statisticians are encouraged to coauthor ecological studies, ensuring statistical methodologies are appropriately tailored to specific research questions. Collaboratively developing new statistical methods to address unique challenges in ecological data, such as spatial autocorrelation, time-series, or high-dimensional datasets, is another important approach. Establishing workshops or consulting services where statisticians provide guidance and feedback on research design, data analysis, and interpretation can further strengthen these interdisciplinary connections. Such collaborations create synergies where statisticians bring technical expertise, and ecologists contribute domain knowledge, leading to more robust and innovative solutions. To institutionalize this practice, research organizations could fund interdisciplinary grants and establish shared positions or joint departments that facilitate these partnerships. By integrating statistical rigor through these collaborations, ecological research can advance with confidence in the validity and reproducibility of its findings, ultimately improving the field's impact and credibility.</p><p>But rather than accepting this state of affairs with collaborative optimism, the field might need to promote changes, and fast. The perpetuation of statistical misconceptions poses a significant challenge in ecological research, often stemming from limited statistical training or misunderstandings of fundamental principles. Researchers may inadvertently propagate incorrect or outdated practices, creating a cascade of flawed methodologies that compromise the integrity of the field. Common issues include the misuse of <i>P</i> values, where an overemphasis on achieving statistical significance (<i>P</i> < 0.05) leads to practices like “p-hacking,” prioritizing significance over meaningful interpretation (Lemoine et al. <span>2016</span>). Similarly, confidence intervals are frequently misunderstood, with many researchers incorrectly assuming that they represent the probability of a parameter falling within the interval, rather than reflecting the uncertainty around an estimate. Another widespread problem is the inappropriate application of statistical tests, such as using parametric methods without verifying assumptions like independence, which can invalidate results. These misconceptions are not mere technical errors; they have far-reaching implications. Flawed analyses can lead to misguided conclusions that undermine decision-making, particularly in contexts where research informs policy or conservation strategies. The reproducibility crisis in science is often linked to these methodological weaknesses, eroding trust in published studies (Jenkins et al. <span>2023</span>). Furthermore, errors in high-profile research can create feedback loops, as early-career researchers often model their approaches on established but flawed examples, perpetuating the cycle of misinformation.</p><p>Another risk is the possible rejection of valid but methodologically innovative studies. Researchers who are not well versed in advanced statistical methods may be skeptical of innovative approaches that challenge traditional practices. This can stifle innovation and prevent the adoption of new and potentially more effective methods. To mitigate this risk, it is important to foster an environment that encourages the exploration of new statistical techniques and supports the rigorous evaluation of their validity. The entrenchment of suboptimal analytical practices is another significant risk. Researchers who rely on familiar but outdated or inappropriate statistical methods may continue to use these methods, even when better alternatives are available. This can lead to the persistence of suboptimal practices and hinder the progress of ecological research. Taking a personal experience as an example, some phylogenetic methods are rapidly expanding, and different methodologies are implemented on a regular basis. Phylogenetic comparative models are powerful tools, but there is a limit to how much we can learn from trees of small size, and this limitation has been widely recognized (Uyeda and Harmon <span>2014</span>). Trying to publish studies with new methods for circumventing these issues may face the challenge of reviewers preferring old and established methods (I am talking about regular PGLS [phylogenetic generalized least squares] models here), despite their inefficiency for certain data characteristics (Caetano and Harmon <span>2019</span>).</p><p>In conclusion, the “statistical competency paradox” in ecological research highlights the need for greater collaboration between ecologists and professional statisticians, the importance of acknowledging our limitations when engaging in methodological debates, the value of even modest statistical knowledge in advancing ecological research, and the potential benefit of enhancing statistical training in ecological education. By addressing these issues, we can improve the robustness and reliability of ecological research and advance the field in a more informed and effective manner. One practical and impactful solution might be implementing mandatory statistical peer review for studies involving advanced statistical methodologies. Under this system, manuscripts employing complex analyses would be reviewed by a qualified statistician in addition to subject matter experts. This process would help ensure that statistical methodologies are rigorously evaluated for appropriateness, validity, and clarity. Furthermore, statistical peer review could identify potential errors or misinterpretations before publication, enhancing the reliability of the findings. While this approach requires additional resources, such as recruiting statisticians and potentially extending review timelines, the benefits outweigh the costs. It would promote a higher standard of statistical rigor, reduce errors in published research, and foster greater trust in the scientific process. To streamline its adoption, journals could create dedicated pools of statistical reviewers and provide guidelines to ensure consistency in reviews. Over time, this practice could elevate the quality of ecological research by embedding robust statistical scrutiny as a standard element of peer review. But these changes are not easy to implement. Journal editors have a particular responsibility here: Not only to mediate methodological disputes but also to actively seek qualified statistical reviewers and encourage author–statistician collaborations.</p><p>In the end, perhaps the real wisdom may lie in recognizing that in ecology, we are all somewhat “blind” when it comes to statistics, and those with slightly better vision should focus on guiding and helping others rather than engaging in territorial disputes about who sees better in the dark. However, this situation presents a significant challenge for the advancement of ecological science as well. While acknowledging our collective statistical limitations is important, mere recognition is insufficient. The current dynamic, where researchers with marginally more statistical knowledge significantly influence methodological decisions, creates potential risks for the field. These include the perpetuation of statistical misconceptions, the possible rejection of valid but methodologically innovative studies, and the entrenchment of suboptimal analytical practices.</p><p>No conflict to declare.</p><p>No data were collected for this study.</p>","PeriodicalId":93418,"journal":{"name":"Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America","volume":"106 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2025-01-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bes2.2230","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/bes2.2230","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
“In the land of the blind, the one-eyed man is king”—this old Dutch proverb carries a deep message about relative advantage and perspective. At its core, the saying suggests that a person with limited knowledge becomes an expert among those who know nothing about the subject or that mediocre talents shine brightly when surrounded by complete beginners. The wisdom of this saying reminds us that value is often relative rather than absolute. It encourages us to recognize that what might seem like a modest capability in one context could be extraordinarily valuable in another. However, there is also a subtle warning within this proverb: what appears to be exceptional in one environment might actually be quite limited in the broader world. The one-eyed person is only “king” in the land of the blind; in a land of people with full vision, they would not hold the same advantage.
This old proverb finds a fascinating parallel in the world of ecological research, particularly regarding statistical analyses and methodological debates during the peer review process. As the editor of an ecology journal myself, I frequently encounter peculiar situations that perfectly illustrate this proverb. I regularly witness intense battles between reviewers and authors over statistical analyses. These methodological debates often become surprisingly heated, with reviewers demanding specific statistical approaches or criticizing authors' analytical choices with remarkable confidence. The authors, in turn, defend their methods with equal vigor or propose alternative analyses with similar conviction. I am an author of scientific papers too and I have been in this battlefield before. But what makes this scenario particularly fascinating is my privileged position as an editor. I can see the academic backgrounds of both the reviewers (who are not anonymous to editors) and the authors, revealing that these passionate statistical arguments are typically occurring between researchers whose primary expertise lies in other areas. For instance, heated debates about the appropriateness of certain probability distributions, statistical packages for specific analyses, the interpretation of main and interactive effects, or the application of mixed models often occur without an adequate understanding of the underlying mathematical principles (Ellison and Dennis 2010). This creates a rather intriguing situation in scientific publishing in ecology: Complex statistical discussions and decisions about methodological rigor are being arbitrated by researchers who, while potentially excellent in their primary field of study, often lack the formal mathematical or statistical training that would traditionally qualify one to make such judgments.
This situation perfectly embodies the proverb's wisdom. Ecologists who have taken a few additional statistics courses or who have spent more time self-studying mathematical concepts often find themselves in positions of significant influence when reviewing or critiquing others' work. Their relatively modest mathematical background, which might seem basic to a trained statistician, becomes a powerful advantage in a field where many researchers have even less statistical training. This dynamic creates what we might term the “statistical competency paradox” in ecological research: While these discussions are crucial for advancing ecological research, they are often conducted by researchers whose statistical expertise is relatively limited compared to actual statisticians (see Chapter 11 in Taper and Lele 2010).
Rather than viewing this situation as a critique of ecological research, we could also see it as an opportunity for growth and improvement. It highlights a unique characteristic of the field: Ecologists must grapple with incredibly complex systems using mathematical tools they were not primarily trained to use. And this challenge is not unique to statistics (Fox 2013). The “one-eyed kings” in these scenarios (those with somewhat more statistical knowledge) may play an important role in raising the bar for methodological rigor, even if their own expertise is limited compared to professional statisticians. While modest statistical proficiency may not suffice for conducting complex analyses, it empowers researchers to make informed methodological choices and collaborate effectively with experts when needed. Thus, on the other hand, even a foundational understanding of statistical principles can hold immense value in ecological research. Researchers with basic statistical knowledge are better equipped to design experiments, interpret results, and critically assess the methodologies employed by others. This competency helps mitigate common errors, such as misinterpretation of statistical tests or misuse of analytical tools, thereby improving the overall reliability of ecological findings. Moreover, it fosters a culture of statistical literacy within the ecological community, bridging the gap between advanced statistical techniques and practical ecological applications. In this sense, even modest statistical knowledge may serve as a cornerstone for robust and impactful ecological research (Barraquand et al. 2014).
However, in the often contentious realm of methodological debates, it is easy to overestimate our expertise (Steel et al. 2013). Researchers, driven by the desire to defend or critique statistical methods, may overlook the limits of their own statistical training. This overconfidence can stifle constructive dialogue and lead to misinformed conclusions. Acknowledging our limitations is not a sign of weakness but a hallmark of scientific integrity. By embracing intellectual humility, researchers create space for meaningful exchanges of ideas and foster a culture of continuous learning. Recognizing gaps in our knowledge encourages us to seek input from those with specialized expertise, ultimately strengthening the rigor of scientific discourse. In this way, humility not only improves the quality of individual contributions but also enhances the collective advancement of science.
To address the statistical competency paradox, it is essential to enhance statistical training in ecological education. This could involve incorporating more advanced statistical courses into ecology programs, encouraging students to take courses in statistics or biostatistics, and providing opportunities for hands-on statistical training (Touchon and McCoy 2016). By equipping ecologists with a stronger foundation in statistics, we can improve the overall quality of ecological research. To meet this demand, academic programs should integrate advanced statistical methods and data analysis courses into core curricula. This integration should be accompanied by hands-on workshops and projects that involve real-world ecological datasets, encouraging students to apply and refine statistical skills in realistic contexts. Additionally, programs should encourage students to take courses in statistics, biostatistics, or data science offered by mathematics or computer science departments, fostering a multidisciplinary perspective. Such initiatives would reduce overreliance on “one-eyed kings” (researchers with limited statistical expertise perceived as experts in their peer groups) and democratize access to statistical knowledge. Moreover, equipping ecologists with advanced statistical tools empowers them to conduct high-quality, independent analyses and critically evaluate the work of others, strengthening the overall research ecosystem.
To bridge gaps in expertise, it is crucial to cultivate stronger collaborations between ecologists and professional statisticians. These partnerships could take several forms, including coauthorship and joint projects where statisticians are encouraged to coauthor ecological studies, ensuring statistical methodologies are appropriately tailored to specific research questions. Collaboratively developing new statistical methods to address unique challenges in ecological data, such as spatial autocorrelation, time-series, or high-dimensional datasets, is another important approach. Establishing workshops or consulting services where statisticians provide guidance and feedback on research design, data analysis, and interpretation can further strengthen these interdisciplinary connections. Such collaborations create synergies where statisticians bring technical expertise, and ecologists contribute domain knowledge, leading to more robust and innovative solutions. To institutionalize this practice, research organizations could fund interdisciplinary grants and establish shared positions or joint departments that facilitate these partnerships. By integrating statistical rigor through these collaborations, ecological research can advance with confidence in the validity and reproducibility of its findings, ultimately improving the field's impact and credibility.
But rather than accepting this state of affairs with collaborative optimism, the field might need to promote changes, and fast. The perpetuation of statistical misconceptions poses a significant challenge in ecological research, often stemming from limited statistical training or misunderstandings of fundamental principles. Researchers may inadvertently propagate incorrect or outdated practices, creating a cascade of flawed methodologies that compromise the integrity of the field. Common issues include the misuse of P values, where an overemphasis on achieving statistical significance (P < 0.05) leads to practices like “p-hacking,” prioritizing significance over meaningful interpretation (Lemoine et al. 2016). Similarly, confidence intervals are frequently misunderstood, with many researchers incorrectly assuming that they represent the probability of a parameter falling within the interval, rather than reflecting the uncertainty around an estimate. Another widespread problem is the inappropriate application of statistical tests, such as using parametric methods without verifying assumptions like independence, which can invalidate results. These misconceptions are not mere technical errors; they have far-reaching implications. Flawed analyses can lead to misguided conclusions that undermine decision-making, particularly in contexts where research informs policy or conservation strategies. The reproducibility crisis in science is often linked to these methodological weaknesses, eroding trust in published studies (Jenkins et al. 2023). Furthermore, errors in high-profile research can create feedback loops, as early-career researchers often model their approaches on established but flawed examples, perpetuating the cycle of misinformation.
Another risk is the possible rejection of valid but methodologically innovative studies. Researchers who are not well versed in advanced statistical methods may be skeptical of innovative approaches that challenge traditional practices. This can stifle innovation and prevent the adoption of new and potentially more effective methods. To mitigate this risk, it is important to foster an environment that encourages the exploration of new statistical techniques and supports the rigorous evaluation of their validity. The entrenchment of suboptimal analytical practices is another significant risk. Researchers who rely on familiar but outdated or inappropriate statistical methods may continue to use these methods, even when better alternatives are available. This can lead to the persistence of suboptimal practices and hinder the progress of ecological research. Taking a personal experience as an example, some phylogenetic methods are rapidly expanding, and different methodologies are implemented on a regular basis. Phylogenetic comparative models are powerful tools, but there is a limit to how much we can learn from trees of small size, and this limitation has been widely recognized (Uyeda and Harmon 2014). Trying to publish studies with new methods for circumventing these issues may face the challenge of reviewers preferring old and established methods (I am talking about regular PGLS [phylogenetic generalized least squares] models here), despite their inefficiency for certain data characteristics (Caetano and Harmon 2019).
In conclusion, the “statistical competency paradox” in ecological research highlights the need for greater collaboration between ecologists and professional statisticians, the importance of acknowledging our limitations when engaging in methodological debates, the value of even modest statistical knowledge in advancing ecological research, and the potential benefit of enhancing statistical training in ecological education. By addressing these issues, we can improve the robustness and reliability of ecological research and advance the field in a more informed and effective manner. One practical and impactful solution might be implementing mandatory statistical peer review for studies involving advanced statistical methodologies. Under this system, manuscripts employing complex analyses would be reviewed by a qualified statistician in addition to subject matter experts. This process would help ensure that statistical methodologies are rigorously evaluated for appropriateness, validity, and clarity. Furthermore, statistical peer review could identify potential errors or misinterpretations before publication, enhancing the reliability of the findings. While this approach requires additional resources, such as recruiting statisticians and potentially extending review timelines, the benefits outweigh the costs. It would promote a higher standard of statistical rigor, reduce errors in published research, and foster greater trust in the scientific process. To streamline its adoption, journals could create dedicated pools of statistical reviewers and provide guidelines to ensure consistency in reviews. Over time, this practice could elevate the quality of ecological research by embedding robust statistical scrutiny as a standard element of peer review. But these changes are not easy to implement. Journal editors have a particular responsibility here: Not only to mediate methodological disputes but also to actively seek qualified statistical reviewers and encourage author–statistician collaborations.
In the end, perhaps the real wisdom may lie in recognizing that in ecology, we are all somewhat “blind” when it comes to statistics, and those with slightly better vision should focus on guiding and helping others rather than engaging in territorial disputes about who sees better in the dark. However, this situation presents a significant challenge for the advancement of ecological science as well. While acknowledging our collective statistical limitations is important, mere recognition is insufficient. The current dynamic, where researchers with marginally more statistical knowledge significantly influence methodological decisions, creates potential risks for the field. These include the perpetuation of statistical misconceptions, the possible rejection of valid but methodologically innovative studies, and the entrenchment of suboptimal analytical practices.
“在盲人之国,独眼人为王”——这句古老的荷兰谚语蕴含着相对优势和远见的深刻含义。这句话的核心意思是,一个知识有限的人在一群对某一学科一无所知的人中成为专家,或者平庸的人才在完全初学者的包围下闪闪发光。这句话的智慧提醒我们,价值往往是相对的,而不是绝对的。它鼓励我们认识到,在一种情况下看似普通的能力,在另一种情况下可能非常有价值。然而,这句谚语中也有一个微妙的警告:在一个环境中看似例外的事情,在更广阔的世界中实际上可能相当有限。独眼的人,在盲人的国度里,只是“王”;在一片充满远见卓识的人民的土地上,他们不会拥有同样的优势。这句古老的谚语在生态研究的世界中发现了一个有趣的相似之处,特别是在同行评审过程中的统计分析和方法辩论方面。作为一家生态学杂志的编辑,我经常遇到一些特殊的情况,这些情况很好地说明了这句谚语。我经常目睹审稿人和作者在统计分析上的激烈斗争。这些方法上的争论经常变得异常激烈,审稿人要求具体的统计方法,或者非常自信地批评作者的分析选择。反过来,作者以同样的活力为自己的方法辩护,或者以同样的信念提出替代分析。我也是一名科学论文的作者,我曾经在这个战场上战斗过。但让这个场景特别吸引人的是我作为编辑的特权地位。我可以看到审稿人(对编辑来说不是匿名的)和作者的学术背景,揭示出这些充满激情的统计争论通常发生在主要专长在其他领域的研究人员之间。例如,在没有充分理解潜在的数学原理的情况下,关于某些概率分布的适当性、特定分析的统计软件包、对主要和交互效应的解释或混合模型的应用的激烈辩论经常发生(Ellison和Dennis 2010)。这在生态学的科学出版中创造了一个相当有趣的情况:复杂的统计讨论和关于方法严密性的决定正在由研究人员进行仲裁,这些研究人员虽然在其主要研究领域可能非常出色,但往往缺乏传统上使人有资格做出此类判断的正式数学或统计训练。这种情况完美地体现了谚语的智慧。选修过一些额外的统计学课程或花更多时间自学数学概念的生态学家经常发现,在审查或批评他人的工作时,自己处于有重大影响的位置。他们相对谦虚的数学背景,对于一个训练有素的统计学家来说可能是基本的,在一个许多研究人员甚至没有受过统计训练的领域成为一个强大的优势。这种动态在生态研究中创造了我们所谓的“统计能力悖论”:虽然这些讨论对于推进生态研究至关重要,但与实际统计学家相比,这些讨论通常是由统计专业知识相对有限的研究人员进行的(见锥度和乐乐2010年的第11章)。与其将这种情况视为对生态研究的批评,我们还不如将其视为一个成长和改进的机会。它突出了该领域的一个独特特征:生态学家必须使用他们最初没有受过训练的数学工具来应对令人难以置信的复杂系统。这一挑战并不是统计学所独有的(Fox 2013)。在这些情况下,“独眼王”(那些拥有更多统计知识的人)可能在提高方法严谨性方面发挥重要作用,即使他们自己的专业知识与专业统计学家相比有限。虽然适度的统计熟练程度可能不足以进行复杂的分析,但它使研究人员能够做出明智的方法选择,并在需要时与专家进行有效合作。因此,另一方面,即使是对统计原理的基本理解也可以在生态学研究中具有巨大的价值。具有基本统计知识的研究人员能够更好地设计实验,解释结果,并批判性地评估其他人采用的方法。这种能力有助于减少常见的错误,例如对统计测试的误解或对分析工具的误用,从而提高生态发现的总体可靠性。 此外,它在生态社区内培养统计知识文化,弥合先进统计技术与实际生态应用之间的差距。从这个意义上说,即使是适度的统计知识也可以作为强大而有影响力的生态研究的基石(Barraquand et al. 2014)。然而,在经常有争议的方法论辩论领域,很容易高估我们的专业知识(Steel et al. 2013)。研究人员在捍卫或批评统计方法的欲望的驱使下,可能会忽视他们自己的统计训练的局限性。这种过度自信会扼杀建设性的对话,导致错误的结论。承认我们的局限性不是软弱的标志,而是科学诚信的标志。通过拥抱智力上的谦逊,研究人员为有意义的思想交流创造了空间,并培养了一种持续学习的文化。认识到我们的知识差距鼓励我们从具有专门知识的人那里寻求意见,最终加强科学论述的严谨性。这样,谦逊不仅提高了个人贡献的质量,而且促进了科学的集体进步。要解决统计能力悖论,必须加强生态教育中的统计培训。这可能包括将更高级的统计课程纳入生态学课程,鼓励学生参加统计学或生物统计学课程,并提供实践统计培训的机会(Touchon和McCoy 2016)。夯实生态学家的统计学基础,可以提高生态学研究的整体质量。为了满足这一需求,学术课程应该将先进的统计方法和数据分析课程纳入核心课程。这种整合应该伴随着涉及现实世界生态数据集的实践研讨会和项目,鼓励学生在现实环境中应用和完善统计技能。此外,项目应鼓励学生参加数学或计算机科学系提供的统计学、生物统计学或数据科学课程,培养多学科视角。这些举措将减少对“独眼王”(在同行中被视为专家的统计专业知识有限的研究人员)的过度依赖,并使统计知识的获取民主化。此外,为生态学家配备先进的统计工具,使他们能够进行高质量的独立分析,并批判性地评估他人的工作,从而加强整个研究生态系统。为了弥合专业知识方面的差距,加强生态学家和专业统计学家之间的合作至关重要。这些伙伴关系可以采取几种形式,包括合作和联合项目,鼓励统计学家共同撰写生态研究,确保统计方法适合特定的研究问题。合作开发新的统计方法来解决生态数据中的独特挑战,如空间自相关、时间序列或高维数据集,是另一个重要的方法。建立研讨会或咨询服务,统计学家在研究设计、数据分析和解释方面提供指导和反馈,可以进一步加强这些跨学科的联系。这样的合作产生了协同效应,统计学家带来了技术专长,生态学家贡献了领域知识,从而产生了更强大和创新的解决方案。为了使这种实践制度化,研究机构可以资助跨学科的资助,并建立共享职位或联合部门来促进这些伙伴关系。通过这些合作整合统计严谨性,生态研究可以对其发现的有效性和可重复性充满信心,最终提高该领域的影响力和可信度。但是,与其以合作的乐观态度接受这种状况,该领域可能需要促进变革,而且要快。统计错误观念的持续存在对生态学研究构成了重大挑战,这些误解往往源于有限的统计训练或对基本原则的误解。研究人员可能会无意中传播不正确或过时的做法,从而产生一系列有缺陷的方法,从而损害该领域的完整性。常见的问题包括滥用P值,过度强调实现统计显著性(P < 0.05)导致“P黑客”等做法,优先考虑显著性而不是有意义的解释(Lemoine et al. 2016)。 同样,置信区间也经常被误解,许多研究人员错误地认为置信区间代表参数落在区间内的概率,而不是反映估计的不确定性。另一个普遍存在的问题是统计检验的不恰当应用,例如使用参数方法而不验证独立性等假设,这可能使结果无效。这些误解不仅仅是技术上的错误;它们具有深远的影响。有缺陷的分析可能导致错误的结论,从而破坏决策,特别是在研究为政策或保护战略提供信息的情况下。科学中的可重复性危机通常与这些方法上的弱点有关,侵蚀了对已发表研究的信任(Jenkins et al. 2023)。此外,高调研究中的错误可能会产生反馈循环,因为早期职业研究人员经常以已建立但有缺陷的例子为模型,使错误信息的循环永久化。另一个风险是可能拒绝有效但方法创新的研究。不精通先进统计方法的研究人员可能对挑战传统做法的创新方法持怀疑态度。这可能会扼杀创新,阻碍采用新的和可能更有效的方法。为了减轻这种风险,重要的是营造一种鼓励探索新的统计技术并支持对其有效性进行严格评估的环境。对次优分析实践的固守是另一个重大风险。依靠熟悉但过时或不适当的统计方法的研究人员可能会继续使用这些方法,即使有更好的替代方法可用。这可能导致次优实践的持续存在,阻碍生态研究的进展。以我个人的经验为例,一些系统发育方法正在迅速扩展,不同的方法正在定期实施。系统发育比较模型是一种强大的工具,但我们能从小型树中学到的东西是有限的,这一限制已经得到了广泛的认可(Uyeda和Harmon 2014)。试图用新方法发表研究来规避这些问题可能会面临审稿人更喜欢旧的和已建立的方法的挑战(我在这里谈论的是常规的PGLS[系统发生广义最小二乘]模型),尽管它们对某些数据特征效率低下(Caetano and Harmon 2019)。总之,生态研究中的“统计能力悖论”强调了生态学家和专业统计学家之间加强合作的必要性,在方法论辩论中承认我们的局限性的重要性,即使是适度的统计知识在推进生态研究中的价值,以及加强生态教育中统计培训的潜在好处。通过解决这些问题,我们可以提高生态研究的稳健性和可靠性,并以更明智和有效的方式推进该领域。一个实际和有效的解决办法可能是对涉及先进统计方法的研究实施强制性的统计同行审查。在这一制度下,除了专题专家外,采用复杂分析的手稿还将由一名合格的统计学家审查。这一过程将有助于确保统计方法的适当性、有效性和明确性得到严格评估。此外,统计同行评议可以在发表前识别潜在的错误或误解,提高研究结果的可靠性。虽然这种方法需要额外的资源,例如招聘统计学家和可能延长审查时间,但收益大于成本。它将促进统计严谨性的更高标准,减少已发表研究中的错误,并促进对科学过程的更大信任。为了简化它的采用,期刊可以创建专门的统计审稿人池,并提供指导方针以确保审稿的一致性。随着时间的推移,这种做法可以通过将强有力的统计审查作为同行评审的标准元素来提高生态研究的质量。但这些变化并不容易实施。期刊编辑在这方面负有特殊的责任:不仅要调解方法上的争议,还要积极寻找合格的统计审稿人,并鼓励作者与统计学家合作。最后,也许真正的智慧在于认识到,在生态学中,当涉及到统计数据时,我们都有点“盲目”,那些视力稍好的人应该专注于指导和帮助他人,而不是参与谁在黑暗中看得更清楚的领土争端。 然而,这种情况也给生态科学的发展带来了重大挑战。虽然承认我们的集体统计局限性很重要,但仅仅承认是不够的。目前的动态是,拥有略微多一点统计知识的研究人员会显著影响方法决策,这给该领域带来了潜在的风险。这些包括统计错误观念的延续,有效但方法上创新的研究可能遭到拒绝,以及次优分析实践的根深蒂固。没有冲突要宣布。本研究未收集数据。