Récupération des fonctions mnésiques et exécutives après un burnout : suivi à un an de patients en arrêt de travail pour épuisement professionnel

IF 0.6 4区 医学 Q4 PSYCHIATRY
Charlie Renaud (Maître de conférences) , Agnès Lacroix (Professeure des universités)
{"title":"Récupération des fonctions mnésiques et exécutives après un burnout : suivi à un an de patients en arrêt de travail pour épuisement professionnel","authors":"Charlie Renaud (Maître de conférences) ,&nbsp;Agnès Lacroix (Professeure des universités)","doi":"10.1016/j.evopsy.2024.08.003","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><div>Numerous studies have demonstrated an association between burnout and cognitive difficulties, yet there is a paucity of longitudinal research on the subject. This study aims to conduct a one-year follow-up on patients who have experienced clinical burnout. Additionally, the literature suggests that variables such as depression, anxiety, neuroticism, emotional regulation strategies, and return to employment may influence cognitive recovery and the patient's perception thereof. Therefore, this study also seeks to delineate the potential roles of these variables in patients’ cognitive recovery.</div></div><div><h3>Method</h3><div>We conducted two assessments with 22 patients (19 women and 3 men), with an average time lapse of 386.10<!--> <!-->days (SD<!--> <!-->=<!--> <!-->20.71) between evaluations. Cognitive functions were assessed using neuropsychological tests. Initially, the MEM-III and GREFEX were employed, while the MEM-IV (the only test altered between the initial encounter and follow-up) was used to assess memory in the second evaluation. The BRIEF-A questionnaire was utilized to evaluate executive functions. Additionally, the French Big Five Inventory, HADS, and the emotion regulation scale were employed to delve into the participants’ psychological profiles.</div></div><div><h3>Results</h3><div>Significant improvements were observed in all memory domains evaluated, encompassing auditory, immediate visual, and delayed visual memory. Regarding executive functions assessed through tests, only four out of sixteen analyses demonstrated significant improvement. These analyses pertained to errors in the interference condition of the Stroop test, categories in the MCST, time for TMT B, and the number of words generated in literal fluency tasks. Self-assessments from the BRIEF-A indicated improvement in the overall executive index and metacognition index. Anxiety and depression levels did not significantly differ one year after the initial assessment. Only burnout and neuroticism levels showed a significant decrease. Furthermore, no differences were observed between patients who had resumed employment and those still seeking employment.</div></div><div><h3>Discussion</h3><div>Memory impairments appear to be temporary and reversible, while executive function data reveal a more nuanced outcome, with a substantial difference between test-assessed recovery and patient self-evaluation. Test results indicate some improvements, particularly in tests assessing inhibition and mental flexibility. However, these improvements may be attributed to increased cognitive endurance, as these tests were administered last in the battery. Patient self-evaluations of executive functions may suggest increased confidence in their cognitive abilities. Exploring the role of self-esteem in the self-assessment of cognitive functions after burnout would be an intriguing avenue for further research. Finally, among all tested co-variables, only neuroticism seems to be associated with cognitive recovery in patients. Depression and anxiety do not consistently appear to have a predominant influence on the link between burnout and cognitive functions.</div></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><div>This study contributes to a nuanced understanding of post-burnout cognitive recovery. Executive and memory functions do not seem to evolve in the same manner. Additionally, the study highlights the importance of patients’ perceptions of their cognitive abilities. These findings underscore the necessity for in-depth research to guide clinical interventions and future investigations in this field.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":45007,"journal":{"name":"Evolution Psychiatrique","volume":"90 1","pages":"Pages 121-133"},"PeriodicalIF":0.6000,"publicationDate":"2024-10-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Evolution Psychiatrique","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0014385524001026","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"PSYCHIATRY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Objectives

Numerous studies have demonstrated an association between burnout and cognitive difficulties, yet there is a paucity of longitudinal research on the subject. This study aims to conduct a one-year follow-up on patients who have experienced clinical burnout. Additionally, the literature suggests that variables such as depression, anxiety, neuroticism, emotional regulation strategies, and return to employment may influence cognitive recovery and the patient's perception thereof. Therefore, this study also seeks to delineate the potential roles of these variables in patients’ cognitive recovery.

Method

We conducted two assessments with 22 patients (19 women and 3 men), with an average time lapse of 386.10 days (SD = 20.71) between evaluations. Cognitive functions were assessed using neuropsychological tests. Initially, the MEM-III and GREFEX were employed, while the MEM-IV (the only test altered between the initial encounter and follow-up) was used to assess memory in the second evaluation. The BRIEF-A questionnaire was utilized to evaluate executive functions. Additionally, the French Big Five Inventory, HADS, and the emotion regulation scale were employed to delve into the participants’ psychological profiles.

Results

Significant improvements were observed in all memory domains evaluated, encompassing auditory, immediate visual, and delayed visual memory. Regarding executive functions assessed through tests, only four out of sixteen analyses demonstrated significant improvement. These analyses pertained to errors in the interference condition of the Stroop test, categories in the MCST, time for TMT B, and the number of words generated in literal fluency tasks. Self-assessments from the BRIEF-A indicated improvement in the overall executive index and metacognition index. Anxiety and depression levels did not significantly differ one year after the initial assessment. Only burnout and neuroticism levels showed a significant decrease. Furthermore, no differences were observed between patients who had resumed employment and those still seeking employment.

Discussion

Memory impairments appear to be temporary and reversible, while executive function data reveal a more nuanced outcome, with a substantial difference between test-assessed recovery and patient self-evaluation. Test results indicate some improvements, particularly in tests assessing inhibition and mental flexibility. However, these improvements may be attributed to increased cognitive endurance, as these tests were administered last in the battery. Patient self-evaluations of executive functions may suggest increased confidence in their cognitive abilities. Exploring the role of self-esteem in the self-assessment of cognitive functions after burnout would be an intriguing avenue for further research. Finally, among all tested co-variables, only neuroticism seems to be associated with cognitive recovery in patients. Depression and anxiety do not consistently appear to have a predominant influence on the link between burnout and cognitive functions.

Conclusion

This study contributes to a nuanced understanding of post-burnout cognitive recovery. Executive and memory functions do not seem to evolve in the same manner. Additionally, the study highlights the importance of patients’ perceptions of their cognitive abilities. These findings underscore the necessity for in-depth research to guide clinical interventions and future investigations in this field.
倦怠后的记忆和执行功能的恢复:对因职业倦怠而停止工作的患者进行一年的监测
许多研究已经证明了倦怠和认知困难之间的联系,但缺乏对这一主题的纵向研究。本研究旨在对临床倦怠患者进行为期一年的随访。此外,文献表明,抑郁、焦虑、神经质、情绪调节策略和重返就业等变量可能影响认知恢复和患者对认知恢复的感知。因此,本研究也试图描述这些变量在患者认知恢复中的潜在作用。方法对22例患者(女性19例,男性3例)进行2次评估,平均间隔时间为386.10 d (SD = 20.71)。使用神经心理学测试评估认知功能。最初,使用memi - iii和GREFEX,而在第二次评估中使用memi - iv(在初次接触和随访之间唯一改变的测试)来评估记忆。使用BRIEF-A问卷评估执行功能。此外,采用法国大五量表、HADS和情绪调节量表对参与者的心理特征进行了深入研究。结果在所有被评估的记忆领域中,包括听觉、即时视觉和延迟视觉记忆,都观察到显著的改善。关于通过测试评估的执行功能,16项分析中只有4项显示出显著改善。这些分析涉及Stroop测试干扰条件的错误、MCST的类别、TMT B的时间和字面流畅性任务产生的单词数量。BRIEF-A的自我评估表明,总体执行指数和元认知指数有所改善。初步评估一年后,焦虑和抑郁水平没有显著差异。只有倦怠和神经质水平有显著下降。此外,在恢复工作的患者和仍在寻找工作的患者之间没有观察到差异。记忆障碍似乎是暂时的和可逆的,而执行功能数据揭示了一个更微妙的结果,在测试评估的恢复和患者自我评估之间存在实质性差异。测试结果显示出一些改善,特别是在评估抑制和心理灵活性的测试中。然而,这些改善可能归因于认知耐力的提高,因为这些测试是在电池中最后进行的。患者对执行功能的自我评估可能表明他们对认知能力的信心有所增强。探讨自尊在职业倦怠后认知功能自我评价中的作用将是一个值得进一步研究的方向。最后,在所有测试的协变量中,只有神经质似乎与患者的认知恢复有关。抑郁和焦虑并不总是对倦怠和认知功能之间的联系产生主要影响。结论本研究有助于对职业倦怠后认知恢复的细致理解。执行和记忆功能似乎不是以同样的方式进化的。此外,该研究强调了患者对自己认知能力的看法的重要性。这些发现强调了深入研究指导临床干预和该领域未来调查的必要性。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
CiteScore
0.70
自引率
50.00%
发文量
72
期刊介绍: Une revue de référence pour le praticien, le chercheur et le étudiant en sciences humaines Cahiers de psychologie clinique et de psychopathologie générale fondés en 1925, Évolution psychiatrique est restée fidèle à sa mission de ouverture de la psychiatrie à tous les courants de pensée scientifique et philosophique, la recherche clinique et les réflexions critiques dans son champ comme dans les domaines connexes. Attentive à histoire de la psychiatrie autant aux dernières avancées de la recherche en biologie, en psychanalyse et en sciences sociales, la revue constitue un outil de information et une source de référence pour les praticiens, les chercheurs et les étudiants.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信