Shalini Gautam , Giang Nguyen , Jonathan Redshaw , Thomas Suddendorf
{"title":"Does the tendency to overestimate future emotions motivate practice in young children?","authors":"Shalini Gautam , Giang Nguyen , Jonathan Redshaw , Thomas Suddendorf","doi":"10.1016/j.cogdev.2024.101515","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Young children and adults tend to overestimate the intensity of their emotional responses to future events. This intensity bias has been proposed to motivate future oriented behaviour. In two studies (N=210) we investigated if the magnitude of the intensity bias was associated with increased practice for an upcoming ‘championship game’. Children (4–8 years) anticipated their emotional response towards losing the championship game, and were then given free time where they could practice in preparation, or play a distractor game. We measured the time children spent playing and the number of attempts they made in the target game. Children reported their emotions once again after they lost the championship game, which was set up to be too difficult to win. Replicating previous findings, children predicted they would feel sadder to lose the game than they ended up reporting actually feeling when they lost. Planned analyses in study 1 revealed no association between the intensity bias and spending more time, or making more attempts, in the target game. However, post hoc analyses found that a greater intensity bias predicted children spending more time per attempt in the target game. This result was replicated in a new sample of children in study 2. We discuss how this finding may support the possibility that the intensity bias functions to motivate future-oriented behaviour.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":51422,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8000,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Cognitive Development","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S088520142400100X","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q3","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstract
Young children and adults tend to overestimate the intensity of their emotional responses to future events. This intensity bias has been proposed to motivate future oriented behaviour. In two studies (N=210) we investigated if the magnitude of the intensity bias was associated with increased practice for an upcoming ‘championship game’. Children (4–8 years) anticipated their emotional response towards losing the championship game, and were then given free time where they could practice in preparation, or play a distractor game. We measured the time children spent playing and the number of attempts they made in the target game. Children reported their emotions once again after they lost the championship game, which was set up to be too difficult to win. Replicating previous findings, children predicted they would feel sadder to lose the game than they ended up reporting actually feeling when they lost. Planned analyses in study 1 revealed no association between the intensity bias and spending more time, or making more attempts, in the target game. However, post hoc analyses found that a greater intensity bias predicted children spending more time per attempt in the target game. This result was replicated in a new sample of children in study 2. We discuss how this finding may support the possibility that the intensity bias functions to motivate future-oriented behaviour.
期刊介绍:
Cognitive Development contains the very best empirical and theoretical work on the development of perception, memory, language, concepts, thinking, problem solving, metacognition, and social cognition. Criteria for acceptance of articles will be: significance of the work to issues of current interest, substance of the argument, and clarity of expression. For purposes of publication in Cognitive Development, moral and social development will be considered part of cognitive development when they are related to the development of knowledge or thought processes.