Ocular vergence under natural conditions. II. Gaze shifts between real targets differing in distance and direction.

C J Erkelens, R M Steinman, H Collewijn
{"title":"Ocular vergence under natural conditions. II. Gaze shifts between real targets differing in distance and direction.","authors":"C J Erkelens,&nbsp;R M Steinman,&nbsp;H Collewijn","doi":"10.1098/rspb.1989.0031","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Horizontal binocular eye movements of three subjects were recorded with the scleral sensor coil--revolving magnetic field technique during voluntary shifts of gaze between pairs of stationary, real, continuously visible targets. The target pairs were located either along the median plane (requiring symmetrical vergence), or on either side of the median plane (requiring asymmetrical vergence). Symmetrical vergence was primarily smooth, but it was often assisted by small, disjunctive saccades. Peak vergence speeds were very high; they increased from about 50 degrees s-1 for vergence changes of 5 degrees to between 150 and 200 degrees s-1 for vergence changes of 34 degrees. Differences between convergence and divergence were idiosyncratic. Asymmetrical vergence, requiring a vergence of 11 degrees combined with a version of 45 degrees, was largely saccadic. Unequal saccades mediated virtually all (95%) of the vergence required in the divergent direction, whereas 75% of the vergence required in the convergent direction was mediated by unequal saccades, with the remaining convergence mediated by smooth vergence, following completion of the saccades. Peak divergence speeds during these saccades were very high (180 degrees s-1 for a change of vergence of 11 degrees); much faster than the smooth, symmetrical vergence change of comparable size (14 degrees). Peak convergent saccadic speeds were about 20% lower. This difference in peak speed was caused by an initial, transient divergence, observed at the beginning of all horizontal saccades. The waveform of disjunctive saccades did not have the same shape as the waveform of conjugate saccades of similar size. The smaller saccade of the disjunctive pair was stretched out in time so as to have the same duration as its larger, companion saccade. These results permitted the conclusion that the subsystems controlling saccades and vergence are not independent. Vergence responses were relatively slow and incomplete with monocular viewing, which excluded disparity as a cue. Monocularly stimulated vergence decreased as a function of the increasing presbyopia of our three subjects. Subjects were able to generate some vergence in darkness towards previously seen and remembered targets. Such responses, however, were slow, irregular and evanescent. In conclusion, vergence shifts between targets, which provided all natural cues to distance, were fast and accurate; they appeared adequate to provide effective binocular vision under natural conditions. This result could not have been expected on the basis of previous observations, all of which had been made with severely reduced cues to depth.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)</p>","PeriodicalId":54561,"journal":{"name":"Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Containing Papers of Abiological Character","volume":"236 1285","pages":"441-65"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1989-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1098/rspb.1989.0031","citationCount":"187","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Containing Papers of Abiological Character","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1989.0031","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 187

Abstract

Horizontal binocular eye movements of three subjects were recorded with the scleral sensor coil--revolving magnetic field technique during voluntary shifts of gaze between pairs of stationary, real, continuously visible targets. The target pairs were located either along the median plane (requiring symmetrical vergence), or on either side of the median plane (requiring asymmetrical vergence). Symmetrical vergence was primarily smooth, but it was often assisted by small, disjunctive saccades. Peak vergence speeds were very high; they increased from about 50 degrees s-1 for vergence changes of 5 degrees to between 150 and 200 degrees s-1 for vergence changes of 34 degrees. Differences between convergence and divergence were idiosyncratic. Asymmetrical vergence, requiring a vergence of 11 degrees combined with a version of 45 degrees, was largely saccadic. Unequal saccades mediated virtually all (95%) of the vergence required in the divergent direction, whereas 75% of the vergence required in the convergent direction was mediated by unequal saccades, with the remaining convergence mediated by smooth vergence, following completion of the saccades. Peak divergence speeds during these saccades were very high (180 degrees s-1 for a change of vergence of 11 degrees); much faster than the smooth, symmetrical vergence change of comparable size (14 degrees). Peak convergent saccadic speeds were about 20% lower. This difference in peak speed was caused by an initial, transient divergence, observed at the beginning of all horizontal saccades. The waveform of disjunctive saccades did not have the same shape as the waveform of conjugate saccades of similar size. The smaller saccade of the disjunctive pair was stretched out in time so as to have the same duration as its larger, companion saccade. These results permitted the conclusion that the subsystems controlling saccades and vergence are not independent. Vergence responses were relatively slow and incomplete with monocular viewing, which excluded disparity as a cue. Monocularly stimulated vergence decreased as a function of the increasing presbyopia of our three subjects. Subjects were able to generate some vergence in darkness towards previously seen and remembered targets. Such responses, however, were slow, irregular and evanescent. In conclusion, vergence shifts between targets, which provided all natural cues to distance, were fast and accurate; they appeared adequate to provide effective binocular vision under natural conditions. This result could not have been expected on the basis of previous observations, all of which had been made with severely reduced cues to depth.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)

自然条件下的眼辐。2目光在距离和方向不同的真实目标之间转移。
用巩膜传感器线圈旋转磁场技术记录了三名受试者在两对静止的、真实的、连续可见的目标之间自主转移目光时的水平双眼眼球运动。目标对要么位于中间平面(需要对称收敛),要么位于中间平面的两侧(需要不对称收敛)。对称的收敛主要是平滑的,但它经常被小的,分离的扫视辅助。峰值收敛速度非常快;它们从5度辐角变化时的50度s-1增加到34度辐角变化时的150到200度s-1。趋同与发散之间的差异是特殊的。不对称的辐角,需要11度的辐角和45度的辐角结合,很大程度上是跳跃式的。不等扫视在发散方向上调解了几乎所有(95%)所需的收敛,而在收敛方向上所需的75%的收敛是由不等扫视调解的,其余的收敛是在扫视完成后由平滑收敛调解的。在这些扫视期间,峰值发散速度非常高(180度s-1,辐辏变化为11度);比同等大小(14度)的平滑、对称的辐合变化快得多。峰值收敛跳跃式速度降低了约20%。这种峰值速度的差异是由在所有水平扫视开始时观察到的初始的、短暂的发散引起的。析取眼跳的波形与相同大小的共轭眼跳波形形状不同。析取对的较小的扫视在时间上被拉长,以便与其较大的伴扫视具有相同的持续时间。这些结果表明控制跳跃和收敛的子系统不是独立的。在单目观察时,视差作为线索被排除在外,辐辏反应相对缓慢且不完整。单眼刺激的聚光随着三位受试者老花眼的增加而减少。实验对象能够在黑暗中对之前看到和记忆中的目标产生一些趋同。然而,这种反应是缓慢的、不规则的和转瞬即逝的。综上所述,目标间的收敛度变化提供了距离的所有自然线索,快速而准确;它们似乎足以在自然条件下提供有效的双目视觉。根据以前的观察,这一结果是不可能预料到的,因为所有这些观察都是在深度线索严重减少的情况下进行的。(摘要删节为400字)
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Containing Papers of Abiological Character
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Containing Papers of Abiological Character 生命科学, 发育生物学与生殖生物学, 发育生物学
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
审稿时长
1 months
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信