Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness Potential

IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES
Walter G. Sannita
{"title":"Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness Potential","authors":"Walter G. Sannita","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000304","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness PotentialWalter G. SannitaWalter G. SannitaWalter G. Sannita, Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health, University of Genova, 3, Largo P. Daneo, 16132 Genova, Italy[email protected] Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics and Mother/child sciences (DINOGMI), University of Genova, Italy Search for more papers by this authorPublished Online:June 27, 2022https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304PDFView Full Text ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInReddit SectionsMoreReferences Bader, F., & Wiener, M. (2021). Awareness of errors and feedback in human time estimation. Learning & Memory, 28(5), 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053108.120 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Binda, P., & Morrone, M. C. (2018). Vision during saccadic eye movements. Annual Review of Vision Science, 4, 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034317 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Boly, M., Seth, A. K., Wilke, M., Ingmundson, P., Baars, B., Laureys, S., Edelman, D. B., & Tsuchiya, N. (2013). Consciousness in humans and non-human animals: Recent advances and future directions. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 625. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00625 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Relative time sharing: New findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1875–1885. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0022 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buzsáki, G., Geisler, C., Henze, D. A., & Wang, X. J. (2004). Interneuron Diversity series: Circuit complexity and axon wiring economy of cortical interneurons. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(4), 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.007 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Celesia, G. G., & Sannita, W. G. (2013). Can patients in vegetative state experience pain and have conscious awareness? Neurology, 80(4), 328–329. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0928 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Cook, N. D., Carvalho, G. B., & Damasio, A. (2014). From membrane excitability to metazoan psychology. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(12), 698–705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.07.011 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar De Corte, B. J., Navarro, V. M., & Wasserman, E. A. (2017). Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons. Current Biology, 27(23), R1264–R1265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.029 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Deecke, L., Weinberg, H., & Brickett, P. (1982). Magnetic fields of the human brain accompanying voluntary movement: Bereitschaftsmagnetfeld. Experimental Brain Research, 48(1), 144–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239582 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Drissi-Daoudi, L., Ögmen, H., Herzog, M. H., & Cicchini, G. M. (2020). Object identity determines trans-saccadic integration. Journal of Vision, 20(7), Article 33. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.7.33 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Droit-Volet, S. (2013). Time perception, emotions and mood disorders. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 107(4), 255–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.005 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Eagleman, D. M. (2008). Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 131–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.002 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Eagleman, D. M., Tse, P. U., Buonomano, D., Janssen, P., Nobre, A. C., & Holcombe, A. O. (2005). Time and the brain: How subjective time relates to neural time. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(45), 10369–10371. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3487-05.2005 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Edelman, D. B., & Seth, A. K. (2009). Animal consciousness: A synthetic approach. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(9), 476–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.008 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Eichenbaum, H. (2017). On the integration of space, time, and memory. Neuron, 95(5), 1007–1018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.036 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Herzog, M. H., Drissi-Daoudi, L., & Doerig, A. (2020). All in good time: Long-lasting postdictive effects reveal discrete perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(10), 826–837. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.07.001 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Herzog, M. H., Kammer, T., & Scharnowski, F. (2016). Time slices: What is the duration of a percept? PLoS Biology, 14(4), Article e1002433. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002433 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Ivry, R. B., & Schlerf, J. E. (2008). Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 273–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Ivry, R. B., & Spencer, R. M. (2004). The neural representation of time. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.013 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Koch, C. (2018). What is consciousness? Scientific American, 318(6), 60–64. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0618-60 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 17(5), 307–321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. R. (2016). Brain potential changes in voluntary and passive movements in humans: readiness potential and reafferent potentials. Pflügers Archiv: European Journal of Physiology, 468(7), 1115–1124. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00424-016-1852-3 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Kotchoubey, B., Vogel, D., Lang, S., & Müller, F. (2014). What kind of consciousness is minimal? Brain Injury, 28(9), 1156–1163. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.920523 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Science, 8(4), 529–566. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00044903 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 106(Pt 3), 623–642. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Madl, T., Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2011). The timing of the cognitive cycle. PLoS One, 6(4), Article e14803. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014803 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Mauk, M. D., & Buonomano, D. V. (2004). The neural basis of temporal processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 307–340. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Meck, W. H., Penney, T. B., & Pouthas, V. (2008). Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 145–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.002 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Merchant, H., Harrington, D. L., & Meck, W. H. (2013). Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 36, 313–336. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Monti, M. M.Sannita, W. G. (Eds.). (2016). Brain function and responsiveness in the disorders of consciousness. Springer. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Morrone, M. C., Cicchini, M., & Burr, D. C. (2010). Spatial maps for time and motion. Experimental Brain Research, 206(2), 121–128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2334-z First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Neafsey, E. J. (2021). Conscious intention and human action: Review of the rise and fall of the readiness potential and Libet’s clock. Consciousness and Cognition, 94, Article 103171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103171 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Ross, J., Morrone, M. C., Goldberg, M. E., & Burr, D. C. (2001). Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(2), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01685-4 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Sannita, W. G. (2008). Neuronal functional diversity and collective behaviors. Journal of Biological Physics, 34(3–4), 267–278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10867-008-9097-9 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Sannita, W. G. (2014). Human brain physiology investigated in the disorder of consciousness. Frontiers in Neurology, 5, Article 211. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2014.00211 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Scharnowski, F., Rees, G., & Walsh, V. (2013). Time and the brain: Neurorelativity: The chronoarchitecture of the brain from the neuronal rather than the observer’s perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(2), 51–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.005 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Schiff, N. D., Ribary, U., Moreno, D. R., Beattie, B., Kronberg, E., Blasberg, R., Giacino, J., McCagg, C., Fins, J. J., Llinás, R., & Plum, F. (2002). Residual cerebral activity and behavioural fragments can remain in the persistently vegetative brain. Brain, 125(Pt 6), 1210–1234. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf131 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Schurger, A., Hu, P., Pak, J., & Roskies, A. L. (2021). What is the readiness potential? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(7), 558–570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.04.001 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Tononi, G., & Koch, C. (2015). Consciousness: Here, there and everywhere? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 370(1668), Article 20140167. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0167 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar White, P. A. (2018). Is conscious perception a series of discrete temporal frames? Consciousness and Cognition, 60, 98–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.02.012 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarFiguresReferencesRelatedDetails Volume 37Issue 2April 2023ISSN: 0269-8803eISSN: 2151-2124 ReceivedDecember 22, 2021RevisedApril 15, 2022AcceptedApril 30, 2022Published onlineJune 27, 2022 InformationJournal of Psychophysiology (2022), 37, pp. 122-124 https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304.© 2022Hogrefe PublishingConflict of Interest:There is no conflict of interest to be disclosed.PDF download Funding: No funding has been requested or obtained.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":"47 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9000,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Journal of Psychophysiology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"NEUROSCIENCES","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness PotentialWalter G. SannitaWalter G. SannitaWalter G. Sannita, Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health, University of Genova, 3, Largo P. Daneo, 16132 Genova, Italy[email protected] Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics and Mother/child sciences (DINOGMI), University of Genova, Italy Search for more papers by this authorPublished Online:June 27, 2022https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304PDFView Full Text ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInReddit SectionsMoreReferences Bader, F., & Wiener, M. (2021). Awareness of errors and feedback in human time estimation. Learning & Memory, 28(5), 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053108.120 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Binda, P., & Morrone, M. C. (2018). Vision during saccadic eye movements. Annual Review of Vision Science, 4, 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034317 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Boly, M., Seth, A. K., Wilke, M., Ingmundson, P., Baars, B., Laureys, S., Edelman, D. B., & Tsuchiya, N. (2013). Consciousness in humans and non-human animals: Recent advances and future directions. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 625. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00625 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Relative time sharing: New findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1875–1885. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0022 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buzsáki, G., Geisler, C., Henze, D. A., & Wang, X. J. (2004). Interneuron Diversity series: Circuit complexity and axon wiring economy of cortical interneurons. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(4), 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.007 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Celesia, G. G., & Sannita, W. G. (2013). Can patients in vegetative state experience pain and have conscious awareness? Neurology, 80(4), 328–329. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0928 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Cook, N. D., Carvalho, G. B., & Damasio, A. (2014). From membrane excitability to metazoan psychology. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(12), 698–705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.07.011 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar De Corte, B. J., Navarro, V. M., & Wasserman, E. A. (2017). Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons. Current Biology, 27(23), R1264–R1265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.029 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Deecke, L., Weinberg, H., & Brickett, P. (1982). Magnetic fields of the human brain accompanying voluntary movement: Bereitschaftsmagnetfeld. Experimental Brain Research, 48(1), 144–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00239582 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Drissi-Daoudi, L., Ögmen, H., Herzog, M. H., & Cicchini, G. M. (2020). Object identity determines trans-saccadic integration. Journal of Vision, 20(7), Article 33. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.7.33 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Droit-Volet, S. (2013). Time perception, emotions and mood disorders. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 107(4), 255–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.005 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Eagleman, D. M. (2008). Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 131–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.002 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Eagleman, D. M., Tse, P. U., Buonomano, D., Janssen, P., Nobre, A. C., & Holcombe, A. O. (2005). Time and the brain: How subjective time relates to neural time. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(45), 10369–10371. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3487-05.2005 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Edelman, D. B., & Seth, A. K. (2009). Animal consciousness: A synthetic approach. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(9), 476–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.008 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Eichenbaum, H. (2017). On the integration of space, time, and memory. Neuron, 95(5), 1007–1018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.036 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Herzog, M. H., Drissi-Daoudi, L., & Doerig, A. (2020). All in good time: Long-lasting postdictive effects reveal discrete perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(10), 826–837. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.07.001 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Herzog, M. H., Kammer, T., & Scharnowski, F. (2016). Time slices: What is the duration of a percept? PLoS Biology, 14(4), Article e1002433. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002433 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Ivry, R. B., & Schlerf, J. E. (2008). Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 273–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Ivry, R. B., & Spencer, R. M. (2004). The neural representation of time. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.013 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Koch, C. (2018). What is consciousness? Scientific American, 318(6), 60–64. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0618-60 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 17(5), 307–321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. R. (2016). Brain potential changes in voluntary and passive movements in humans: readiness potential and reafferent potentials. Pflügers Archiv: European Journal of Physiology, 468(7), 1115–1124. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00424-016-1852-3 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Kotchoubey, B., Vogel, D., Lang, S., & Müller, F. (2014). What kind of consciousness is minimal? Brain Injury, 28(9), 1156–1163. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.920523 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Science, 8(4), 529–566. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00044903 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 106(Pt 3), 623–642. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Madl, T., Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2011). The timing of the cognitive cycle. PLoS One, 6(4), Article e14803. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014803 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Mauk, M. D., & Buonomano, D. V. (2004). The neural basis of temporal processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 307–340. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Meck, W. H., Penney, T. B., & Pouthas, V. (2008). Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 145–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.002 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Merchant, H., Harrington, D. L., & Meck, W. H. (2013). Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 36, 313–336. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Monti, M. M.Sannita, W. G. (Eds.). (2016). Brain function and responsiveness in the disorders of consciousness. Springer. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Morrone, M. C., Cicchini, M., & Burr, D. C. (2010). Spatial maps for time and motion. Experimental Brain Research, 206(2), 121–128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2334-z First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Neafsey, E. J. (2021). Conscious intention and human action: Review of the rise and fall of the readiness potential and Libet’s clock. Consciousness and Cognition, 94, Article 103171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103171 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Ross, J., Morrone, M. C., Goldberg, M. E., & Burr, D. C. (2001). Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(2), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01685-4 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Sannita, W. G. (2008). Neuronal functional diversity and collective behaviors. Journal of Biological Physics, 34(3–4), 267–278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10867-008-9097-9 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Sannita, W. G. (2014). Human brain physiology investigated in the disorder of consciousness. Frontiers in Neurology, 5, Article 211. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2014.00211 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Scharnowski, F., Rees, G., & Walsh, V. (2013). Time and the brain: Neurorelativity: The chronoarchitecture of the brain from the neuronal rather than the observer’s perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(2), 51–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.005 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Schiff, N. D., Ribary, U., Moreno, D. R., Beattie, B., Kronberg, E., Blasberg, R., Giacino, J., McCagg, C., Fins, J. J., Llinás, R., & Plum, F. (2002). Residual cerebral activity and behavioural fragments can remain in the persistently vegetative brain. Brain, 125(Pt 6), 1210–1234. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf131 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Schurger, A., Hu, P., Pak, J., & Roskies, A. L. (2021). What is the readiness potential? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(7), 558–570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.04.001 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Tononi, G., & Koch, C. (2015). Consciousness: Here, there and everywhere? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 370(1668), Article 20140167. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0167 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar White, P. A. (2018). Is conscious perception a series of discrete temporal frames? Consciousness and Cognition, 60, 98–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.02.012 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarFiguresReferencesRelatedDetails Volume 37Issue 2April 2023ISSN: 0269-8803eISSN: 2151-2124 ReceivedDecember 22, 2021RevisedApril 15, 2022AcceptedApril 30, 2022Published onlineJune 27, 2022 InformationJournal of Psychophysiology (2022), 37, pp. 122-124 https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304.© 2022Hogrefe PublishingConflict of Interest:There is no conflict of interest to be disclosed.PDF download Funding: No funding has been requested or obtained.
时间、意识和准备潜力
(2008)。时间感知的专用和内在模型。认知科学趋势,12(7),273-280。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002在文章ecrossref中的首次引用,Google Scholar Ivry, R. B.和Spencer, R. M.(2004)。时间的神经表征。神经生物学杂志,14(2),225-232。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.013在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者Koch, C.(2018)。什么是意识?科学美国人,318(6),60-64。https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0618-60文章ecrossref首次引用,谷歌学者Koch, C, Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G.(2016)。意识的神经关联:进展与问题。自然评论:神经科学,17(5),307-321。https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22文章ecrossref首次引用,谷歌学者Kornhuber, h.h., & Deecke, l.r.(2016)。人类主动和被动运动中的脑电位变化:准备电位和再传入电位。[j] .中国生物医学工程学报,2016,33(4):559 - 564。https://doi.org/10.1007/s00424-016-1852-3文章首次被引用于ecrossref,谷歌学者Kotchoubey, B., Vogel, D., Lang, S., & m<e:1> ller, F.(2014)。什么样的意识是最小的?脑损伤,28(9),1156-1163。https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.920523在文章ecrossref中首次引用,Google Scholar Libet, B.(1985)。无意识的大脑主动性和有意识的意志在自愿行动中的作用。心理科学学报,2009(4):529 - 536。https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00044903在文章ecrossref中的首次引用,Google Scholar Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K.(1983)。与大脑活动的开始有关的有意识的行动意图的时间(准备-电位)。无意识地开始一种自由自愿的行为。中国生物医学工程学报,2009(3),623-642。https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623文章首次被引用于ecrossref, Google Scholar Madl, T., Baars, b.j., & Franklin, S.(2011)。认知周期的时间。科学通报,6(4),Article e14803。https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014803在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者Mauk, m.d, & Buonomano, d.v.(2004)。时间处理的神经基础。神经科学年鉴,27,307-340。https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247文章首次引用于ecrosref, Google Scholar Meck, W. H., Penney, T. B., & Pouthas, V.(2008)。动物和人类的皮质纹状体时间表征。神经生物学杂志,18(2),145-152。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.002文章首次引用ecrosref, Google Scholar Merchant, H., Harrington, D. L, & Meck, W. H.(2013)。感知和估计时间的神经基础。神经科学进展,36,313-336。https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349文章首次引用ecrosref,谷歌学者Monti, m.m.s annita, w.g.(编辑)。(2016)。意识障碍中的脑功能和反应。施普林格。文章首次引用于ecrosref, Google Scholar Morrone, m.c., Cicchini, m.m., & Burr, d.c.(2010)。时间和运动的空间地图。脑科学进展,2016(2),391 - 391。https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2334-z在文章ecrossref中首次引用,Google Scholar Neafsey, E. J.(2021)。有意识的意图和人类行为:战备潜力的起落和利贝特时钟的回顾。《意识与认知》,1994,第103171条。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103171在文章ecrossref中的首次引用,谷歌学者Ross, J., Morrone, m.c., Goldberg, m.e., & Burr, d.c.(2001)。扫视时视觉感知的变化。神经科学进展,24(2),113-121。https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01685-4在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者sanita, w.g.(2008)。神经元功能多样性与集体行为。生物物理学报,34(3),267-278。https://doi.org/10.1007/s10867-008-9097-9在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者sanita, w.g.(2014)。在意识障碍中研究人脑生理学。神经病学前沿,5,第211篇。https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2014.00211在ecrossref中首次被引用,谷歌学者Scharnowski, F., Rees, G., & Walsh, V.(2013)。时间与大脑:神经相对论:从神经元而非观察者的角度来研究大脑的时间结构。认知科学动态,17(2),51-52。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.005文章首次引用ecrosref, Google Scholar Schiff, N. D., Ribary, U., Moreno, D. R., Beattie, B., Kronberg, E., Blasberg, R., Giacino, J., McCagg, C., Fins, J., Llinás, R., & Plum, F.(2002)。残留的大脑活动和行为碎片可以留在持续的植物人大脑中。脑科学,125(6),1210-1234。https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf131文章首引ecrossref, Google Scholar Schurger, A., Hu, P., Pak, J.。 , & rosky, a.l.(2021)。准备潜力是多少?认知科学趋势,25(7),558-570。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.04.001在文章ecrossref中首次引用,Google Scholar Tononi, G., & Koch, C.(2015)。意识:这里,那里和无处不在?伦敦皇家学会哲学汇刊:B辑,生物科学,370(1668),第20140167条。https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0167文章首引google Scholar White, p.a.(2018)。有意识的知觉是一系列离散的时间框架吗?意识与认知,60,98-126。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.02.012首次引用于article eccrossref, Google ScholarFiguresReferencesRelatedDetails Volume 37Issue 2April 2023ISSN: 0269-8803eISSN:2151-2124收于2021年12月22日修订于2022年4月15日接受于2022年4月30日在线发布于2022年6月27日信息《心理生理学杂志》(2022),37,pp. 122-124 https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304.©2022Hogrefe publishing利益冲突:无利益冲突需要披露。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
Journal of Psychophysiology
Journal of Psychophysiology 医学-神经科学
CiteScore
2.60
自引率
7.70%
发文量
25
审稿时长
>12 weeks
期刊介绍: The Journal of Psychophysiology is an international periodical that presents original research in all fields employing psychophysiological measures on human subjects. Contributions are published from psychology, physiology, clinical psychology, psychiatry, neurosciences, and pharmacology. Communications on new psychophysiological methods are presented as well. Space is also allocated for letters to the editor and book reviews. Occasional special issues are devoted to important current issues in psychophysiology.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信