The Politics of Intergroup Attitudes

Brian A. Nosek, M. Banaji, J. Jost
{"title":"The Politics of Intergroup Attitudes","authors":"Brian A. Nosek, M. Banaji, J. Jost","doi":"10.1093/ACPROF:OSO/9780195320916.003.020","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Ideologies that underlie concepts of ethnocentrism, authoritarianism, system justification, social dominance, and morality shape minds in sufficiently deep ways to bring about (a) congruence between implicit and explicit preferences, and (b) a consistently greater preference for socially advantaged groups among political conservatives than liberals on both explicit and implicit measures. Data from large web samples and representative samples from the American National Election Studies (ANES) provide support for these and two additional results: (a) liberals show greater mean dissociation between explicit and implicit attitudes than conservatives, reporting more favorable attitudes toward the underprivileged groups than they demonstrate on implicit measures; and (b) over time, conservatives’ racial preferences converge on those of liberals, suggesting that where liberals are today, conservatives will be tomorrow. Intergroup Attitudes 2 The Politics of Intergroup Attitudes Intergroup attitudes are made up of complex strands of social preferences. They are held together by political ideologies that serve as orienting systems guiding personality as well as responses to the environment such as decisions about the information one chooses to consume, the activities one pursues, and the policies one supports (Jost, 2006). They are sufficiently central to social cognition that they are visible in the the neural markers that distinguish a politically similar other from one who is dissimilar (Mitchell, Macrae & Banaji, 2006). In this chapter we rely on two large datasets that provide substantial evidence regarding attitudes toward multiple social groups (e.g., groups based on religion, sexuality, ethnicity/race, age, and gender). From these data we examine the role of political ideology as an organizing concept for the structure and function of social attitudes; simultaneously, we examine intergroup attitudes to understand more about the liberal-conservative (or left-right) political divide. In the last two decades, the idea that attitudes, like other mental processes, may reside in both conscious/explicit as well as less conscious/implicit form has come to be well-accepted (e.g., Bargh, 1997; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). This distinction in attitudes may apply to philosophical and ideological belief systems as well (Jost, 2006; Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004; Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003). That is, political ideology – an interconnected set of beliefs and attitudes that shape judgment – may not exist solely as a reasoned or explicit collection of beliefs and attitudes. Ideology has unconscious as well as conscious determinants, and the latter is well explicated elsewhere (Cunningham, Nezlek & Banaji, 2004; see Ferguson, Carter, & Hassin, this volume). In this chapter, we examine the variation in ideological orientation in relation to implicit and explicit attitudes, with a specific focus on attitudes toward social groups. We start by revisiting Jost et al.’s (2003) theoretical argument (and supporting meta-analysis) that liberals and conservatives differ on two key dimensions: resistance to change and tolerance for inequality. We then focus on the key prediction that conservatives are more likely than liberals to have and express more positive attitudes toward high-status or advantaged groups and more negative attitudes toward low-status or disadvantaged groups. We will describe that this is true on both conscious and less conscious measures of intergroup attitudes. In this sense, the intergroup attitudes of conservatives tend to be more system-justifying than those of liberals insofar as they support and perpetuate the existing social hierarchy (see also Jost et al., 2004; Jost, Nosek, & Gosling, in press). We also find--using data from the American National Election Studies (ANES)--that liberals are at the forefront of the social movement toward racial egalitarianism, whereas conservatives’ attitudes were slower to change. Ideological Differences between Liberals and Conservatives The “classic” conception of the authoritarian personality (Adorno, FrenkelBrunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950), its modern instantiation (Altemeyer, Intergroup Attitudes 3 1996), recent perspectives on ideology stressing motivated social cognition (Jost et al., 2003), system justification (Jost & Banaji, 1994; Jost et al., 2004, in press), social dominance (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999), and moral foundations (Haidt & Graham, 2007) provide accounts of differences between the political left and right, or, more commonly in the U.S., liberalism and conservatism. These perspectives converge on the expectation that, compared to liberals, conservatives are less concerned with equality, more comfortable maintaining the status quo, and more likely to show favoritism for high-status or advantaged groups over low-status or disadvantaged groups. To the extent that conservative, system-justifying attitudes are characterized by resistance to change and tolerance for inequality (Jost et al., 2003), their appeal should be maximized when stability and order are prioritized values. In the study of authoritarianism, psychologists have long observed that societal crises (e.g., economic upheavals, terrorist attacks) often precipitate rightward political shifts, presumably because conservative, right-wing opinions typically resonate with heightened needs to manage uncertainty and threat (Doty, Peterson, & Winter, 1991; Sales, 1972, 1973; McCann, 1997; Ullrich & Cohrs, 2007). As Huntington (1957) put it, “When the foundations of society are threatened, the conservative ideology reminds men of the necessity of some institutions and desirability of the existing ones.” In other words, system-level threats stimulate the motivation to justify the system. A meta-analytic review of the psychological antecedents of political conservatism by Jost et al. (2003) supports this view. Specifically, they found that situational as well as dispositional variables associated with the management of threat and uncertainty predicted various manifestations of political conservatism (including economic system justification). The original studies were conducted in 12 countries between 1958 and 2002 and employed 88 different research samples involving a total of 22,818 individual cases. Results indicated that the tendency to endorse conservative (rather than liberal or moderate) opinions is positively associated with threat variables such as mortality salience (or death anxiety), system instability, and fear of threat and loss, and it is negatively associated (albeit weakly) with self-esteem. Conservatism is also positively associated with uncertainty avoidance, intolerance of ambiguity, and needs for order, structure, and closure, and it is negatively associated with openness to experience and integrative complexity. Although the meta-analysis focused on explicit, self-reported attitudes and beliefs, recent research using implicit measures mirrors these ideological differences. For example, ideological differences in resistance to change were demonstrated by Jost, Nosek, and Gosling (in press), who found that implicit and explicit attitudes toward tradition, stability, and the status quo were predictors of political orientation. More specifically, conservatism was associated with greater implicit as well as explicit preferences for order compared to chaos, conformity compared to rebelliousness, stability compared to flexibility, tradition compared to progress, and traditional values compared to feminism. In simultaneous regressions, both implicit and explicit attitudes showed unique predictive validity of political orientation, suggesting that they are non-redundant indicators of Intergroup Attitudes 4 ideological proclivities. Ideological differences in implicit social cognition also relate to the tolerance of inequality and, specifically, favoritism for higher over lower status groups. Jost, Banaji, and Nosek (2004) found that individual differences in political orientation moderated implicit attitudes for social groups. Measured with the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), conservatives, compared to liberals, showed stronger preferences for White Americans over African Americans and for heterosexuals over homosexuals (see also Cunningham, Nezlek, & Banaji, 2004; see also Sidanius & Pratto, 1999 for similar results at the explicit level). The meta-analysis and subsequent investigations of implicit social cognition provide an initial basis for the notion that conservatives are more likely to show favoritism for higher than lower status groups than liberals, on both implicit and explicit measures. We examined this possibility across a variety of target groups with nationally representative samples of voters collected through the American National Election Studies (ANES) and with large datasets collected over the Internet. Measuring Ideology and Implicit Attitudes Validity of a single-item political ideology assessment Remarkably, even the simplest of questions – self-placement on a singleitem liberal to conservative dimension – appears to be an effective means of parsing individual differences in ideological orientations. Jost (2006) analyzed American National Election Studies data from 1972 to 2004 and found that a selfplacement on a 7-point single item of strongly liberal to strongly conservative explained 85% of the variance in voting behavior for Democratic and Republican candidates for president. Similar evidence is available from large datasets showing that selfplacement on a liberal-conservative dimension discriminates both explicit and implicit attitudes toward politicians. Nosek and colleagues (Nosek et al., 2007) summarized approximately six years’ worth of data collected at Project Implicit web sites (see https://implicit.harvard.edu/). The aggregated datasets included more than 2.5 million IATs and self-reported attitude assessments acr","PeriodicalId":296540,"journal":{"name":"The Cambridge Handbook of Political Psychology","volume":"33 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2009-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"13","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The Cambridge Handbook of Political Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1093/ACPROF:OSO/9780195320916.003.020","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13

Abstract

Ideologies that underlie concepts of ethnocentrism, authoritarianism, system justification, social dominance, and morality shape minds in sufficiently deep ways to bring about (a) congruence between implicit and explicit preferences, and (b) a consistently greater preference for socially advantaged groups among political conservatives than liberals on both explicit and implicit measures. Data from large web samples and representative samples from the American National Election Studies (ANES) provide support for these and two additional results: (a) liberals show greater mean dissociation between explicit and implicit attitudes than conservatives, reporting more favorable attitudes toward the underprivileged groups than they demonstrate on implicit measures; and (b) over time, conservatives’ racial preferences converge on those of liberals, suggesting that where liberals are today, conservatives will be tomorrow. Intergroup Attitudes 2 The Politics of Intergroup Attitudes Intergroup attitudes are made up of complex strands of social preferences. They are held together by political ideologies that serve as orienting systems guiding personality as well as responses to the environment such as decisions about the information one chooses to consume, the activities one pursues, and the policies one supports (Jost, 2006). They are sufficiently central to social cognition that they are visible in the the neural markers that distinguish a politically similar other from one who is dissimilar (Mitchell, Macrae & Banaji, 2006). In this chapter we rely on two large datasets that provide substantial evidence regarding attitudes toward multiple social groups (e.g., groups based on religion, sexuality, ethnicity/race, age, and gender). From these data we examine the role of political ideology as an organizing concept for the structure and function of social attitudes; simultaneously, we examine intergroup attitudes to understand more about the liberal-conservative (or left-right) political divide. In the last two decades, the idea that attitudes, like other mental processes, may reside in both conscious/explicit as well as less conscious/implicit form has come to be well-accepted (e.g., Bargh, 1997; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). This distinction in attitudes may apply to philosophical and ideological belief systems as well (Jost, 2006; Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004; Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003). That is, political ideology – an interconnected set of beliefs and attitudes that shape judgment – may not exist solely as a reasoned or explicit collection of beliefs and attitudes. Ideology has unconscious as well as conscious determinants, and the latter is well explicated elsewhere (Cunningham, Nezlek & Banaji, 2004; see Ferguson, Carter, & Hassin, this volume). In this chapter, we examine the variation in ideological orientation in relation to implicit and explicit attitudes, with a specific focus on attitudes toward social groups. We start by revisiting Jost et al.’s (2003) theoretical argument (and supporting meta-analysis) that liberals and conservatives differ on two key dimensions: resistance to change and tolerance for inequality. We then focus on the key prediction that conservatives are more likely than liberals to have and express more positive attitudes toward high-status or advantaged groups and more negative attitudes toward low-status or disadvantaged groups. We will describe that this is true on both conscious and less conscious measures of intergroup attitudes. In this sense, the intergroup attitudes of conservatives tend to be more system-justifying than those of liberals insofar as they support and perpetuate the existing social hierarchy (see also Jost et al., 2004; Jost, Nosek, & Gosling, in press). We also find--using data from the American National Election Studies (ANES)--that liberals are at the forefront of the social movement toward racial egalitarianism, whereas conservatives’ attitudes were slower to change. Ideological Differences between Liberals and Conservatives The “classic” conception of the authoritarian personality (Adorno, FrenkelBrunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950), its modern instantiation (Altemeyer, Intergroup Attitudes 3 1996), recent perspectives on ideology stressing motivated social cognition (Jost et al., 2003), system justification (Jost & Banaji, 1994; Jost et al., 2004, in press), social dominance (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999), and moral foundations (Haidt & Graham, 2007) provide accounts of differences between the political left and right, or, more commonly in the U.S., liberalism and conservatism. These perspectives converge on the expectation that, compared to liberals, conservatives are less concerned with equality, more comfortable maintaining the status quo, and more likely to show favoritism for high-status or advantaged groups over low-status or disadvantaged groups. To the extent that conservative, system-justifying attitudes are characterized by resistance to change and tolerance for inequality (Jost et al., 2003), their appeal should be maximized when stability and order are prioritized values. In the study of authoritarianism, psychologists have long observed that societal crises (e.g., economic upheavals, terrorist attacks) often precipitate rightward political shifts, presumably because conservative, right-wing opinions typically resonate with heightened needs to manage uncertainty and threat (Doty, Peterson, & Winter, 1991; Sales, 1972, 1973; McCann, 1997; Ullrich & Cohrs, 2007). As Huntington (1957) put it, “When the foundations of society are threatened, the conservative ideology reminds men of the necessity of some institutions and desirability of the existing ones.” In other words, system-level threats stimulate the motivation to justify the system. A meta-analytic review of the psychological antecedents of political conservatism by Jost et al. (2003) supports this view. Specifically, they found that situational as well as dispositional variables associated with the management of threat and uncertainty predicted various manifestations of political conservatism (including economic system justification). The original studies were conducted in 12 countries between 1958 and 2002 and employed 88 different research samples involving a total of 22,818 individual cases. Results indicated that the tendency to endorse conservative (rather than liberal or moderate) opinions is positively associated with threat variables such as mortality salience (or death anxiety), system instability, and fear of threat and loss, and it is negatively associated (albeit weakly) with self-esteem. Conservatism is also positively associated with uncertainty avoidance, intolerance of ambiguity, and needs for order, structure, and closure, and it is negatively associated with openness to experience and integrative complexity. Although the meta-analysis focused on explicit, self-reported attitudes and beliefs, recent research using implicit measures mirrors these ideological differences. For example, ideological differences in resistance to change were demonstrated by Jost, Nosek, and Gosling (in press), who found that implicit and explicit attitudes toward tradition, stability, and the status quo were predictors of political orientation. More specifically, conservatism was associated with greater implicit as well as explicit preferences for order compared to chaos, conformity compared to rebelliousness, stability compared to flexibility, tradition compared to progress, and traditional values compared to feminism. In simultaneous regressions, both implicit and explicit attitudes showed unique predictive validity of political orientation, suggesting that they are non-redundant indicators of Intergroup Attitudes 4 ideological proclivities. Ideological differences in implicit social cognition also relate to the tolerance of inequality and, specifically, favoritism for higher over lower status groups. Jost, Banaji, and Nosek (2004) found that individual differences in political orientation moderated implicit attitudes for social groups. Measured with the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), conservatives, compared to liberals, showed stronger preferences for White Americans over African Americans and for heterosexuals over homosexuals (see also Cunningham, Nezlek, & Banaji, 2004; see also Sidanius & Pratto, 1999 for similar results at the explicit level). The meta-analysis and subsequent investigations of implicit social cognition provide an initial basis for the notion that conservatives are more likely to show favoritism for higher than lower status groups than liberals, on both implicit and explicit measures. We examined this possibility across a variety of target groups with nationally representative samples of voters collected through the American National Election Studies (ANES) and with large datasets collected over the Internet. Measuring Ideology and Implicit Attitudes Validity of a single-item political ideology assessment Remarkably, even the simplest of questions – self-placement on a singleitem liberal to conservative dimension – appears to be an effective means of parsing individual differences in ideological orientations. Jost (2006) analyzed American National Election Studies data from 1972 to 2004 and found that a selfplacement on a 7-point single item of strongly liberal to strongly conservative explained 85% of the variance in voting behavior for Democratic and Republican candidates for president. Similar evidence is available from large datasets showing that selfplacement on a liberal-conservative dimension discriminates both explicit and implicit attitudes toward politicians. Nosek and colleagues (Nosek et al., 2007) summarized approximately six years’ worth of data collected at Project Implicit web sites (see https://implicit.harvard.edu/). The aggregated datasets included more than 2.5 million IATs and self-reported attitude assessments acr
群体间态度的政治
作为种族中心主义、威权主义、制度正当化、社会支配和道德等概念基础的意识形态以足够深刻的方式塑造了人们的思想,从而导致(a)内隐偏好和外显偏好之间的一致性,以及(b)政治保守派比自由主义者在外显和内隐方面始终更倾向于社会优势群体。来自大型网络样本和来自美国全国选举研究(ANES)的代表性样本的数据为这些和两个额外的结果提供了支持:(a)自由主义者比保守主义者在显式和隐性态度之间表现出更大的平均分离,报告对弱势群体的态度比他们在隐性测量中表现出的更有利;(b)随着时间的推移,保守派的种族偏好与自由派趋同,这意味着今天自由派在哪里,明天保守派就会在哪里。群体间态度群体间态度是由一系列复杂的社会偏好构成的。它们由政治意识形态结合在一起,作为指导人格的导向系统以及对环境的反应,例如关于一个人选择消费的信息,一个人追求的活动和一个人支持的政策的决定(Jost, 2006)。它们是社会认知的核心,在区分政治上相似和不同的人的神经标记中是可见的(Mitchell, Macrae & Banaji, 2006)。在本章中,我们依赖于两个大型数据集,这些数据集提供了关于对多个社会群体(例如,基于宗教、性别、种族/种族、年龄和性别的群体)的态度的实质性证据。从这些数据中,我们考察了政治意识形态作为社会态度结构和功能的组织概念的作用;同时,我们研究了群体间的态度,以更多地了解自由-保守(或左右)的政治分歧。在过去的二十年里,态度和其他心理过程一样,可能以意识/外显和非意识/内隐两种形式存在的观点已经被广泛接受(例如,Bargh, 1997;Greenwald & Banaji, 1995)。这种态度上的区别也可能适用于哲学和意识形态信仰体系(Jost, 2006;Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004;Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003)。也就是说,政治意识形态——形成判断的一系列相互关联的信仰和态度——可能并不仅仅作为一种理性的或明确的信仰和态度的集合而存在。意识形态既有无意识的决定因素,也有有意识的决定因素,后者在其他地方得到了很好的解释(Cunningham, Nezlek & Banaji, 2004;见Ferguson, Carter, & Hassin,本卷)。在本章中,我们研究了与内隐和外显态度有关的意识形态取向的变化,并特别关注对社会群体的态度。我们首先回顾Jost等人(2003)的理论论点(并支持荟萃分析),即自由派和保守派在两个关键维度上存在差异:抵制变革和容忍不平等。然后,我们将重点放在关键预测上,即保守派比自由派更有可能对高地位或优势群体持积极态度,而对低地位或弱势群体持消极态度。我们将描述这在群体间态度的有意识和不那么有意识的测量中都是正确的。从这个意义上说,保守主义者的群体间态度比自由主义者更倾向于系统合理化,因为他们支持并延续现有的社会等级制度(参见Jost et al., 2004;约斯特、诺塞克和高斯林出版)。我们还发现——使用来自美国全国选举研究(ANES)的数据——自由主义者处于走向种族平等主义的社会运动的前沿,而保守派的态度变化较慢。权威人格的“经典”概念(Adorno, FrenkelBrunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950),它的现代实例(Altemeyer, Intergroup Attitudes 3 1996),强调动机社会认知的意识形态的最新观点(Jost等人,2003),制度辩护(Jost & Banaji, 1994;Jost et al., 2004, in press),社会支配地位(Sidanius & Pratto, 1999)和道德基础(Haidt & Graham, 2007)提供了政治左派和右派之间的差异,或者,在美国更常见的是自由主义和保守主义之间的差异。这些观点都集中在这样一种预期上:与自由主义者相比,保守主义者更不关心平等,更愿意维持现状,更有可能对地位高或优势群体表现出偏爱,而不是地位低或弱势群体。 保守的、为制度辩护的态度以抵制变革和容忍不平等为特征(Jost et al., 2003),当稳定和秩序被优先考虑时,它们的吸引力应该最大化。在对威权主义的研究中,心理学家长期以来观察到,社会危机(如经济动荡、恐怖袭击)往往会促使政治转向右翼,大概是因为保守的右翼观点通常会与管理不确定性和威胁的高度需求产生共鸣(Doty, Peterson, & Winter, 1991;销售,1972年,1973年;麦肯,1997;Ullrich & Cohrs, 2007)。正如亨廷顿(1957)所言:“当社会的基础受到威胁时,保守主义意识形态提醒人们某些制度的必要性和现有制度的可取性。”换句话说,系统级威胁刺激了证明系统合理性的动机。Jost等人(2003)对政治保守主义心理前因的荟萃分析支持这一观点。具体来说,他们发现与威胁和不确定性管理相关的情境变量和性格变量预测了政治保守主义的各种表现(包括经济制度辩护)。最初的研究是在1958年至2002年间在12个国家进行的,采用了88个不同的研究样本,总共涉及22,818个个案。结果表明,倾向于支持保守(而不是自由或温和)的观点与死亡显著性(或死亡焦虑)、系统不稳定性、对威胁和损失的恐惧等威胁变量呈正相关,与自尊呈负相关(尽管弱)。保守主义还与不确定性规避、不容忍歧义、对秩序、结构和封闭的需求呈正相关,与经验开放性和综合复杂性呈负相关。尽管荟萃分析关注的是明确的、自我报告的态度和信仰,但最近的研究使用了内隐测量方法,反映了这些意识形态差异。例如,Jost, Nosek和Gosling(出版)证明了抵制变革的意识形态差异,他们发现对传统,稳定和现状的含蓄和明确态度是政治倾向的预测因素。更具体地说,保守主义与更大的隐性和显性偏好有关,即秩序与混乱相比,顺从与反叛相比,稳定与灵活相比,传统与进步相比,传统价值观与女权主义相比。在同步回归中,内隐态度和外显态度都显示出独特的政治倾向预测有效性,表明它们是群体间态度4意识形态倾向的非冗余指标。内隐社会认知的意识形态差异也与对不平等的容忍度有关,特别是对地位较高的群体的偏爱。Jost, Banaji, and Nosek(2004)发现政治取向的个体差异会调节社会群体的内隐态度。内隐联想测验(IAT);Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998),与自由主义者相比,保守主义者对美国白人的偏好高于非裔美国人,对异性恋者的偏好高于同性恋者(另见Cunningham, Nezlek, & Banaji, 2004;参见Sidanius & Pratto, 1999在显式层面上的类似结果)。内隐社会认知的元分析和后续调查为保守主义者比自由主义者更倾向于高地位群体而不是低地位群体这一概念提供了初步的基础,无论是内隐还是外显措施。我们通过美国国家选举研究(ANES)收集的具有全国代表性的选民样本和通过互联网收集的大型数据集,在各种目标群体中检验了这种可能性。值得注意的是,即使是最简单的问题——在自由到保守的单项维度上自我定位——似乎也是分析个人意识形态取向差异的有效手段。Jost(2006)分析了1972年至2004年的美国全国选举研究数据,发现在强烈自由主义到强烈保守主义的7分单项上的自我定位解释了民主党和共和党总统候选人投票行为差异的85%。从大型数据集中可以获得类似的证据,表明在自由-保守维度上的自我定位区分了对政治家的显性和隐性态度。Nosek和他的同事(Nosek et al, 2007)总结了大约6年的隐式项目网站收集的数据(见https://implicit.harvard.edu/)。 汇总的数据集包括250多万份个人调查问卷和自我报告的态度评估问卷
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信