Auditory Images As Input For Speech Recognition Systems

R. Patterson, J. Holdsworth, P. Thurston, T. Robinson
{"title":"Auditory Images As Input For Speech Recognition Systems","authors":"R. Patterson, J. Holdsworth, P. Thurston, T. Robinson","doi":"10.1109/ASPAA.1991.634090","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Over the past decade, hearing scientists have developed a number of time-domain models of the processing performed by the cochlea in an effort to develop a reasonably accurate multi-channel representation of the pattern of neural activity flowing from the cochlea up the auditory nerve to the cochlear nucleus [l]. It is often assumed that peripheral auditory processing ends at the output of the cochlea and that the pattern of activity in the auditory nerve is in some sense what we hear. In reality, this neural activity pattern (NAP) is not a good representation of our auditory sensations because it includes phase differences that we do riot hear and it does not include auditory temporal integration (TI). As a result, several of the models have been extended to include periodicity-sensitive TI [2], [3], [4] which converts the fast-flowing neural activity pattern into a form that is much more like the auditory images we experience in response to sounds. When these models are applied to speech sounds, the auditory images of vowels reveal an elaborate formant structure that is absent in the more traditional representation of speech -the spectrogram. An example is presented on the left in the figure; it is the auditory image of the stationary part of the vowel /ae/ as in 'bab' [4]. The abscissa of the auditory image is 'temporal integration interval' and each line of the image shows the activity in one frequency channel of the auditory model. In general terms, activity on a vertical line in the auditory image shows that there is a correlation in the sound at that temporal interval. The coincentrations of activity are the formants of the vowel.","PeriodicalId":146017,"journal":{"name":"Final Program and Paper Summaries 1991 IEEE ASSP Workshop on Applications of Signal Processing to Audio and Acoustics","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"1","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Final Program and Paper Summaries 1991 IEEE ASSP Workshop on Applications of Signal Processing to Audio and Acoustics","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1109/ASPAA.1991.634090","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1

Abstract

Over the past decade, hearing scientists have developed a number of time-domain models of the processing performed by the cochlea in an effort to develop a reasonably accurate multi-channel representation of the pattern of neural activity flowing from the cochlea up the auditory nerve to the cochlear nucleus [l]. It is often assumed that peripheral auditory processing ends at the output of the cochlea and that the pattern of activity in the auditory nerve is in some sense what we hear. In reality, this neural activity pattern (NAP) is not a good representation of our auditory sensations because it includes phase differences that we do riot hear and it does not include auditory temporal integration (TI). As a result, several of the models have been extended to include periodicity-sensitive TI [2], [3], [4] which converts the fast-flowing neural activity pattern into a form that is much more like the auditory images we experience in response to sounds. When these models are applied to speech sounds, the auditory images of vowels reveal an elaborate formant structure that is absent in the more traditional representation of speech -the spectrogram. An example is presented on the left in the figure; it is the auditory image of the stationary part of the vowel /ae/ as in 'bab' [4]. The abscissa of the auditory image is 'temporal integration interval' and each line of the image shows the activity in one frequency channel of the auditory model. In general terms, activity on a vertical line in the auditory image shows that there is a correlation in the sound at that temporal interval. The coincentrations of activity are the formants of the vowel.
听觉图像作为语音识别系统的输入
在过去的十年里,听力科学家已经开发了许多耳蜗处理过程的时域模型,试图对从耳蜗沿听神经流向耳蜗核的神经活动模式建立一个合理准确的多通道表示[1]。人们通常认为外周听觉处理结束于耳蜗的输出,并且在某种意义上,听觉神经的活动模式就是我们听到的。实际上,这种神经活动模式(NAP)并不能很好地代表我们的听觉,因为它包含了我们听不到的相位差,而且它不包括听觉时间整合(TI)。因此,一些模型已经扩展到包括周期性敏感的TI[2],[3],[4],它将快速流动的神经活动模式转换成一种更像我们在响应声音时所经历的听觉图像的形式。当这些模型应用于语音时,元音的听觉图像揭示了一个复杂的形成峰结构,这在更传统的语音表示中是不存在的——谱图。在图的左边给出了一个例子;它是元音/ae/静止部分的听觉形象,如在'bab'[4]中。听觉图像的横坐标是“时间整合间隔”,图像的每一行表示听觉模型的一个频率通道的活动。一般来说,听觉图像中竖线上的活动表明在那个时间间隔内声音之间存在相关性。活动的集中是元音的共振峰。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信