How Large Are Cognitive Gender Differences?

J. Hyde
{"title":"How Large Are Cognitive Gender Differences?","authors":"J. Hyde","doi":"10.4324/9780429035302-10","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Maccoby and Jacklin concluded that the following cognitive gender differences are well-established: verbal ability, quantitative ability, and visualspatial ability. The present study applied meta-analysis techniques to the studies cited by Maccoby and Jacklin, assessing the magnitude of gender differences using both o> and A statistics. The results indicated that gender differences in all of these abilities are very small: For verbal ability, the median u? was .01 and the median d was .24; for quantitative ability, the median values of (a and d were .01 and .43, respectively; for visualspatial ability, they were .043 and .45, respectively; and for field articulation, to was .025 and d was .51. Discussion focused on the practical implications of the finding that these \"well-established\" differences were in fact very small. Concerns about sampling were raised. The problem was also discussed in the context of a larger issue in psychological research: the limitations of the hypothesis-testing approach and the need to estimate the magnitude of effects. Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) reviewed the enormous literature on psychological gender differences. In particular, they concluded that three cognitive gender differences were \"well-established\": Girls have greater verbal ability than boys, and boys have better visual-spatial ability and better mathematical ability than girls. Sherman (1978) re-reviewed the evidence on cognitive gender differences and pointed out that even for these supposedly well-established differences, the magnitude of the gender difference was very small. For example, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974, Table 3.4) computed the magnitude of gender differences in verbal ability for a subset of studies providing sufficient data. Typically, the magnitude of the difference was only about .25 standard deviations. Sherman (1978, p. 43) noted that the proportion of variance (w) accounted for by gender differences in verbal ability for the 1955 892 • AUGUST 1981 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST standardization of the WAIS (Matarazzo, 1972) was less than .01: Meta-analysis is a technique for analyzing a body of research on a particular topic by statistical analysis of the analyses of the individual studies (Glass, 1976). It is becoming increasingly popular as a technique for evaluating a given area of research (for examples, see Hall, 1978; Kulik, Kulik, & Cohen, 1979; Smith, 1980; Smith & Glass, 1977). Typically, these studies use the measure d = MI M2 ' —as a measure of the magnitude of difoU ferences between two groups, that is, as a measure of effect size. The purpose of the present article is to reanalyze the studies on cognitive gender differences considered to be well-established by Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) and to determine the magnitude of these gender differences. Two measures of effect size were used: co (Hays, 1963) and d. Such an analysis is particularly important because the term \"well-established\" is often taken to mean \"large.\" Maccoby and Jacklin's review has had a widespread influence, and their conclusions are cited in many introductory-level psychology texts. Thus, large numbers of people may have the impression that there are large gender differences in cognitive abilities. This, in turn, may affect practices such as vocational counseling. For example, a girl might be discouraged from a career in mathematics or science because of the \"well-established\" and \"large\" superiority of males in quantitative ability (despite repeated caveats in many texts about the great overlap in male and female distributions). Thus, it seems important to determine whether Requests for reprints should be sent to Janet Shibley Hyde, Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville, Ohio 43023. Vol. 36, No. 8, 892-901 Copyright 1981 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. (K)03-066X/81/3608-0892$00.75 these \"well-established\" differences are in fact \"large.\" This study can be viewed as one example of a larger issue in psychological research: criticism of the hypothesis-testing approach and a suggested alternative, estimating the magnitude of effects. Cognitive gender differences are good examples because they are widely believed to be well-established, yet the size of the gender difference has rarely been estimated.","PeriodicalId":319045,"journal":{"name":"Feminist Research Methods","volume":"154 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1981-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"79","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Feminist Research Methods","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429035302-10","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 79

Abstract

Maccoby and Jacklin concluded that the following cognitive gender differences are well-established: verbal ability, quantitative ability, and visualspatial ability. The present study applied meta-analysis techniques to the studies cited by Maccoby and Jacklin, assessing the magnitude of gender differences using both o> and A statistics. The results indicated that gender differences in all of these abilities are very small: For verbal ability, the median u? was .01 and the median d was .24; for quantitative ability, the median values of (a and d were .01 and .43, respectively; for visualspatial ability, they were .043 and .45, respectively; and for field articulation, to was .025 and d was .51. Discussion focused on the practical implications of the finding that these "well-established" differences were in fact very small. Concerns about sampling were raised. The problem was also discussed in the context of a larger issue in psychological research: the limitations of the hypothesis-testing approach and the need to estimate the magnitude of effects. Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) reviewed the enormous literature on psychological gender differences. In particular, they concluded that three cognitive gender differences were "well-established": Girls have greater verbal ability than boys, and boys have better visual-spatial ability and better mathematical ability than girls. Sherman (1978) re-reviewed the evidence on cognitive gender differences and pointed out that even for these supposedly well-established differences, the magnitude of the gender difference was very small. For example, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974, Table 3.4) computed the magnitude of gender differences in verbal ability for a subset of studies providing sufficient data. Typically, the magnitude of the difference was only about .25 standard deviations. Sherman (1978, p. 43) noted that the proportion of variance (w) accounted for by gender differences in verbal ability for the 1955 892 • AUGUST 1981 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST standardization of the WAIS (Matarazzo, 1972) was less than .01: Meta-analysis is a technique for analyzing a body of research on a particular topic by statistical analysis of the analyses of the individual studies (Glass, 1976). It is becoming increasingly popular as a technique for evaluating a given area of research (for examples, see Hall, 1978; Kulik, Kulik, & Cohen, 1979; Smith, 1980; Smith & Glass, 1977). Typically, these studies use the measure d = MI M2 ' —as a measure of the magnitude of difoU ferences between two groups, that is, as a measure of effect size. The purpose of the present article is to reanalyze the studies on cognitive gender differences considered to be well-established by Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) and to determine the magnitude of these gender differences. Two measures of effect size were used: co (Hays, 1963) and d. Such an analysis is particularly important because the term "well-established" is often taken to mean "large." Maccoby and Jacklin's review has had a widespread influence, and their conclusions are cited in many introductory-level psychology texts. Thus, large numbers of people may have the impression that there are large gender differences in cognitive abilities. This, in turn, may affect practices such as vocational counseling. For example, a girl might be discouraged from a career in mathematics or science because of the "well-established" and "large" superiority of males in quantitative ability (despite repeated caveats in many texts about the great overlap in male and female distributions). Thus, it seems important to determine whether Requests for reprints should be sent to Janet Shibley Hyde, Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville, Ohio 43023. Vol. 36, No. 8, 892-901 Copyright 1981 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. (K)03-066X/81/3608-0892$00.75 these "well-established" differences are in fact "large." This study can be viewed as one example of a larger issue in psychological research: criticism of the hypothesis-testing approach and a suggested alternative, estimating the magnitude of effects. Cognitive gender differences are good examples because they are widely believed to be well-established, yet the size of the gender difference has rarely been estimated.
认知上的性别差异有多大?
麦科比和杰克林得出结论,以下认知上的性别差异是公认的:语言能力、定量能力和视觉空间能力。本研究将荟萃分析技术应用于Maccoby和Jacklin引用的研究,使用o>和A统计来评估性别差异的程度。结果表明,所有这些能力的性别差异都非常小:在语言能力方面,中位数的平均值为6.6%。为0.01,中位数d为0.24;在定量能力方面,(a)和(d)的中位数分别为0.01和0.43;视觉空间能力分别为0.043和0.45;对于野外关节,to为0.025,d为0.51。讨论集中在这些“公认的”差异实际上非常小这一发现的实际含义上。有人提出了对抽样的关切。这个问题还在心理学研究中一个更大的问题的背景下进行了讨论:假设检验方法的局限性和估计影响程度的必要性。Maccoby和Jacklin(1974)回顾了大量关于心理性别差异的文献。他们特别得出结论,三个认知性别差异是“公认的”:女孩比男孩有更好的语言能力,男孩比女孩有更好的视觉空间能力和数学能力。Sherman(1978)重新审视了关于认知性别差异的证据,并指出,即使对于这些公认的差异,性别差异的幅度也非常小。例如,Maccoby和Jacklin (1974, Table 3.4)在提供足够数据的研究子集中计算了语言能力的性别差异程度。通常情况下,差异的大小只有0.25个标准差。Sherman(1978,第43页)指出,在1955年892年至1981年8月美国心理学家标准化的WAIS (Matarazzo, 1972)中,语言能力的性别差异所占的方差比例(w)小于0.01:元分析是一种通过对个别研究分析的统计分析来分析特定主题的研究主体的技术(Glass, 1976)。作为一种评估特定研究领域的技术,它正变得越来越受欢迎(例如,见Hall, 1978;库利克,库利克和科恩,1979;史密斯,1980;史密斯与格拉斯出版社,1977)。通常,这些研究使用度量d = MI M2 ' -作为两组之间差异大小的度量,即作为效应大小的度量。本文的目的是重新分析Maccoby和Jacklin(1974)对认知性别差异的研究,并确定这些性别差异的程度。使用了效应大小的两种测量方法:co (Hays, 1963)和d。这种分析特别重要,因为术语“行之有效”通常意味着“大”。Maccoby和Jacklin的评论产生了广泛的影响,他们的结论在许多入门级的心理学文本中被引用。因此,很多人可能会有这样的印象,即认知能力存在很大的性别差异。反过来,这可能会影响职业咨询等实践。例如,一个女孩可能不愿意从事数学或科学方面的职业,因为男性在定量能力方面“公认的”和“巨大的”优势(尽管在许多文本中反复警告男女分布的巨大重叠)。因此,决定是否将重印请求发送给Janet Shibley Hyde, Denison大学心理学系,Granville, Ohio 43023似乎很重要。这些“公认的”差异实际上是“很大的”。这项研究可以看作是心理学研究中一个更大问题的一个例子:对假设检验方法的批评和建议的替代方法,估计影响的程度。认知上的性别差异就是一个很好的例子,因为人们普遍认为性别差异是确定的,然而性别差异的大小却很少被估计出来。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信