Look Before You Leap: Representativeness of Those Completing Self-Reports in Early Psychosis Research

S. Iyer, S. Mustafa, S. Abadi, R. Joober, A. Malla
{"title":"Look Before You Leap: Representativeness of Those Completing Self-Reports in Early Psychosis Research","authors":"S. Iyer, S. Mustafa, S. Abadi, R. Joober, A. Malla","doi":"10.1177/0706743719879356","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"A randomized controlled trial (RCT) was conducted to compare the efficacy of early intervention (EI) for psychosis extended for 5 years with 2 years of EI followed by 3 years of regular care (details in prior publications). As secondary hypotheses, we postulated that the extended EI group (n 1⁄4 110) would have higher levels of alliance with their treatment providers than the control group (n 1⁄4 110) and that differences in medication adherence and retention in treatment between the two groups would be predicted by working alliance. Alliance was to be measured every 6 months after randomization (i.e., Months 30, 36, 42, 48, 54, and 60) with the Working Alliance Inventory (WAI), a self-report instrument. The average working alliance in the extended EI and regular care groups was 63.53 (SD 1⁄4 12.24, range: 24 to 84, N 1⁄4 85) and 59.35 (SD 1⁄4 12.30, range: 30 to 82, N 1⁄4 46), respectively, t(129) 1⁄4 1.862, P 1⁄4 0.065. The minimum score on the WAI is 12 and the maximum 84, indicating that in both arms, individuals reported moderate to high levels of alliance. Our results should not be interpreted as indicative of a true lack of difference in therapeutic alliance between persons receiving extended EI and regular care, given that individuals in the EI group were significantly likelier to have filled out the WAI at least once during follow-up than those receiving regular care (83% vs.44%, respectively; w 1⁄4 36.129, P < 0.001). We therefore chose not to conduct any additional analyses of the impact of alliance on group differences in medication adherence and retention. Willingness to fill out self-reports may in itself serve as an indicator of alliance/engagement with mental health systems/teams. Earlier, we reported that individuals receiving extended EI were likelier to remain engaged in their followup and have more contacts with their doctors and other treatment providers. That they were also likelier to complete the WAI may be an additional indicator of better “engagement” in the EI group. Assessments were conducted in both arms by the same research assistant. Nonetheless, some factors may have facilitated completion of measures in the EI group. Selfreports and structured interviews to assess symptoms were completed during in-person appointments with the research assistant who was in the same institution as the EI program, albeit in a separate pavilion. When individuals could not come in person, the research assistant completed the structured symptom interviews on the telephone. Self-reports, however, could not be done telephonically. This may have contributed to a higher rate of completion of symptom assessments (our primary outcome). If individuals consented, self-report measures were mailed to them, but very few mailed measures were returned. In the context of an RCT, it was important for the research assistant to be blind to treatment condition. This impeded us from adopting methods that prior research including in psychosis suggests may have facilitated completion of self-reports, for example, asking people to complete selfreports during their clinical appointments, so that completed measures could inform treatment decision-making in real time. Because a sizable number of individuals did not ever fill out the WAI, we examined the differences between individuals who completed the WAI during at least one of the six time points (n1⁄4 139) versus those who never completed it at any time point (n 1⁄4 81), irrespective of treatment condition. Although 139 individuals filled out the measure, 8 left out individual items, resulting in a total of 131 individuals for whom average WAI could be calculated. There was no significant difference between those who completed the WAI","PeriodicalId":309115,"journal":{"name":"The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry","volume":"104 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743719879356","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

Abstract

A randomized controlled trial (RCT) was conducted to compare the efficacy of early intervention (EI) for psychosis extended for 5 years with 2 years of EI followed by 3 years of regular care (details in prior publications). As secondary hypotheses, we postulated that the extended EI group (n 1⁄4 110) would have higher levels of alliance with their treatment providers than the control group (n 1⁄4 110) and that differences in medication adherence and retention in treatment between the two groups would be predicted by working alliance. Alliance was to be measured every 6 months after randomization (i.e., Months 30, 36, 42, 48, 54, and 60) with the Working Alliance Inventory (WAI), a self-report instrument. The average working alliance in the extended EI and regular care groups was 63.53 (SD 1⁄4 12.24, range: 24 to 84, N 1⁄4 85) and 59.35 (SD 1⁄4 12.30, range: 30 to 82, N 1⁄4 46), respectively, t(129) 1⁄4 1.862, P 1⁄4 0.065. The minimum score on the WAI is 12 and the maximum 84, indicating that in both arms, individuals reported moderate to high levels of alliance. Our results should not be interpreted as indicative of a true lack of difference in therapeutic alliance between persons receiving extended EI and regular care, given that individuals in the EI group were significantly likelier to have filled out the WAI at least once during follow-up than those receiving regular care (83% vs.44%, respectively; w 1⁄4 36.129, P < 0.001). We therefore chose not to conduct any additional analyses of the impact of alliance on group differences in medication adherence and retention. Willingness to fill out self-reports may in itself serve as an indicator of alliance/engagement with mental health systems/teams. Earlier, we reported that individuals receiving extended EI were likelier to remain engaged in their followup and have more contacts with their doctors and other treatment providers. That they were also likelier to complete the WAI may be an additional indicator of better “engagement” in the EI group. Assessments were conducted in both arms by the same research assistant. Nonetheless, some factors may have facilitated completion of measures in the EI group. Selfreports and structured interviews to assess symptoms were completed during in-person appointments with the research assistant who was in the same institution as the EI program, albeit in a separate pavilion. When individuals could not come in person, the research assistant completed the structured symptom interviews on the telephone. Self-reports, however, could not be done telephonically. This may have contributed to a higher rate of completion of symptom assessments (our primary outcome). If individuals consented, self-report measures were mailed to them, but very few mailed measures were returned. In the context of an RCT, it was important for the research assistant to be blind to treatment condition. This impeded us from adopting methods that prior research including in psychosis suggests may have facilitated completion of self-reports, for example, asking people to complete selfreports during their clinical appointments, so that completed measures could inform treatment decision-making in real time. Because a sizable number of individuals did not ever fill out the WAI, we examined the differences between individuals who completed the WAI during at least one of the six time points (n1⁄4 139) versus those who never completed it at any time point (n 1⁄4 81), irrespective of treatment condition. Although 139 individuals filled out the measure, 8 left out individual items, resulting in a total of 131 individuals for whom average WAI could be calculated. There was no significant difference between those who completed the WAI
三思而后行:早期精神病研究中完成自我报告的代表性
进行了一项随机对照试验(RCT),比较早期干预(EI)治疗延长5年的精神病与2年的EI后3年的常规护理的疗效(详情见先前的出版物)。作为次要假设,我们假设扩展EI组(n 1⁄4 110)与治疗提供者的联盟水平高于对照组(n 1⁄4 110),并且两组在药物依从性和治疗保留方面的差异可以通过工作联盟来预测。随机化后每6个月(即第30、36、42、48、54和60个月)使用自我报告工具Working Alliance Inventory (WAI)测量一次联盟。扩展EI组和常规护理组的平均工作联盟分别为63.53 (SD 1⁄4 12.24,范围:24 ~ 84,N 1⁄4 85)和59.35 (SD 1⁄4 12.30,范围:30 ~ 82,N 1⁄4 46),t(129) 1⁄4 1.862,P 1⁄4 0.065。WAI的最低得分为12分,最高得分为84分,表明在两支队伍中,个体报告了中等到高度的联盟。我们的研究结果不应该被解释为表明接受延长EI和常规护理的患者之间的治疗联盟确实缺乏差异,因为EI组的个体在随访期间至少有一次填写WAI的可能性明显高于接受常规护理的个体(分别为83%对44%;w 1 / 4 36.129, P < 0.001)。因此,我们选择不进行任何额外的分析联盟对药物依从性和保留的组差异的影响。愿意填写自我报告本身可以作为与精神卫生系统/团队的联盟/参与的指标。早些时候,我们报告说,接受扩展EI的个体更有可能继续参与他们的随访,并与他们的医生和其他治疗提供者有更多的联系。他们也更有可能完成WAI,这可能是EI组更好的“参与”的另一个指标。两组的评估由同一名研究助理进行。尽管如此,一些因素可能促进了EI组测量的完成。自我报告和评估症状的结构化访谈是在与研究助理的面谈中完成的,该助理与EI项目在同一机构,尽管在不同的展馆。当个体无法亲自前来时,研究助理通过电话完成结构化的症状访谈。然而,自我报告不能通过电话进行。这可能有助于提高症状评估的完成率(我们的主要结果)。如果个人同意,自我报告量表将被邮寄给他们,但很少有邮寄的量表被退回。在随机对照试验中,研究助理对治疗条件不知情是很重要的。这阻碍了我们采用先前的研究方法,包括在精神病方面的研究表明,这些方法可能有助于完成自我报告,例如,要求人们在临床预约期间完成自我报告,以便完成的测量可以实时告知治疗决策。由于相当数量的个体从未填写过WAI,我们检查了在六个时间点中至少一个时间点(n1⁄4 139)完成WAI的个体与在任何时间点(n1⁄4 81)从未完成WAI的个体之间的差异,无论治疗情况如何。虽然有139人填写了该量表,但有8人遗漏了个人项目,因此总共有131人可以计算平均WAI。完成WAI的人之间没有显著差异
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术官方微信