Article 28

B. Jorgensen, Mathew A. Wilson, T. Heberlein
{"title":"Article 28","authors":"B. Jorgensen, Mathew A. Wilson, T. Heberlein","doi":"10.5771/9783845279893-1093","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"This paper analyzes \"protest responses,\" or the reasons why people are unwilling to pay to obtain (or avoid) an increase (or decrease) in some environmental public good when surveyed in studies using the contingent valuation method. This presents a problem to this method because it does not necessarily indicate they do not value a public good, but simply it is someone else's responsibility to pay, they already pay enough, there is too much governmental waste, etc. Censoring such responses leads to unrepresentative samples of the population or problems with validity in the collected data if inconsistent data is ignored since many beliefs are not independent of one another. Respondents might be unwilling to pay for any public good for reasons unrelated to how they value a specific improvement, are unable due to budgetary constraints despite desire to do so, or only if it is for a local improvement and on a small enough scale to make a difference. To test the effects of protest responses' effects, a contingent valuation survey was conducted in northern Wisconsin. Respondents were asked about their willingness to pay for four environmental public goods (biodiversity, spearfishing, water quality, and wolves) at two levels of scope (part and whole). For water quality and spearfishing, the \"part\" level of scope were a chain of lakes that were considered among all lakes, at the \"whole\" level of scope, in Oneida and Vilas counties. For biodiversity, the \"part\" level was Oneida and Vilas counties while the \"whole\" was all of northern Wisconsin. The \"part\" level for wolves consisted of introducing 300 wolves while the \"whole\" level was introducing 800 wolves. People were more inclined to pay for biodiversity (69%) and water quality (62%) than wolves (51%) and Indian spearfishing (44%) at the partial level, with 3% unwilling or unable to pay. As a whole public good, a similar result was observed (biodiversity, 72%; water quality, 73%; wolves, 47%; Indian spearfishing, 49%), with 7% unwilling or unable to pay. Some demographic variability also existed for each public good category. In general, wolf protection was not as worthy as a spending priority and not considered a personal rights issue like the other three. It is clear that water quality and biodiversity are also more seriously considered priorities for household spending than wolves and spearfishing. While the distribution of protest beliefs was sensitive to the type of public good being valued, it was less sensitive to the scope of public good change. The higher percentage of people willing to pay for the public good as a whole indicates people are probably more likely to pay more money to preserve a greater amount of the good rather than less to preserve only part of it.","PeriodicalId":249042,"journal":{"name":"UN Convention on Contracts for the International Sale of Goods (CISG)","volume":"19 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"1993-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"25","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"UN Convention on Contracts for the International Sale of Goods (CISG)","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.5771/9783845279893-1093","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 25

Abstract

This paper analyzes "protest responses," or the reasons why people are unwilling to pay to obtain (or avoid) an increase (or decrease) in some environmental public good when surveyed in studies using the contingent valuation method. This presents a problem to this method because it does not necessarily indicate they do not value a public good, but simply it is someone else's responsibility to pay, they already pay enough, there is too much governmental waste, etc. Censoring such responses leads to unrepresentative samples of the population or problems with validity in the collected data if inconsistent data is ignored since many beliefs are not independent of one another. Respondents might be unwilling to pay for any public good for reasons unrelated to how they value a specific improvement, are unable due to budgetary constraints despite desire to do so, or only if it is for a local improvement and on a small enough scale to make a difference. To test the effects of protest responses' effects, a contingent valuation survey was conducted in northern Wisconsin. Respondents were asked about their willingness to pay for four environmental public goods (biodiversity, spearfishing, water quality, and wolves) at two levels of scope (part and whole). For water quality and spearfishing, the "part" level of scope were a chain of lakes that were considered among all lakes, at the "whole" level of scope, in Oneida and Vilas counties. For biodiversity, the "part" level was Oneida and Vilas counties while the "whole" was all of northern Wisconsin. The "part" level for wolves consisted of introducing 300 wolves while the "whole" level was introducing 800 wolves. People were more inclined to pay for biodiversity (69%) and water quality (62%) than wolves (51%) and Indian spearfishing (44%) at the partial level, with 3% unwilling or unable to pay. As a whole public good, a similar result was observed (biodiversity, 72%; water quality, 73%; wolves, 47%; Indian spearfishing, 49%), with 7% unwilling or unable to pay. Some demographic variability also existed for each public good category. In general, wolf protection was not as worthy as a spending priority and not considered a personal rights issue like the other three. It is clear that water quality and biodiversity are also more seriously considered priorities for household spending than wolves and spearfishing. While the distribution of protest beliefs was sensitive to the type of public good being valued, it was less sensitive to the scope of public good change. The higher percentage of people willing to pay for the public good as a whole indicates people are probably more likely to pay more money to preserve a greater amount of the good rather than less to preserve only part of it.
第二十八条
本文分析了“抗议反应”,即在使用条件评估方法进行调查的研究中,人们不愿意为获得(或避免)增加(或减少)某些环境公共产品而付费的原因。这对这种方法提出了一个问题,因为它不一定表明他们不重视公共产品,而只是其他人有责任支付,他们已经付出了足够的钱,政府浪费太多了,等等。如果忽略不一致的数据,因为许多信念不是相互独立的,那么审查这些回答会导致人口样本不具代表性,或者在所收集的数据中存在有效性问题。受访者可能不愿意为任何公共产品支付费用,原因与他们如何评价特定的改进无关,尽管希望这样做,但由于预算限制而无法支付,或者只有在局部改进并且规模足够小以产生影响时才会这样做。为了检验抗议反应的效果,在威斯康辛州北部进行了一项条件评估调查。受访者被问及他们是否愿意在两个层面(部分和整体)为四种环境公共产品(生物多样性、鱼叉捕鱼、水质和狼)付费。对于水质和鱼叉捕鱼,范围的“部分”水平是在所有湖泊中考虑的湖泊链,在范围的“整体”水平,在奥内达和维拉斯县。生物多样性的“部分”水平是奥内达县和维拉斯县,而“整体”水平是威斯康辛州北部的所有县。狼的“部分”关卡包括引入300只狼,而“整体”关卡则是引入800只狼。在部分层面上,人们更倾向于为生物多样性(69%)和水质(62%)付费,而不是为狼(51%)和印第安鱼叉捕鱼(44%)付费,3%的人不愿意或无法付费。作为一个整体的公共产品,观察到类似的结果(生物多样性,72%;水质,73%;狼,47%;印度人用鱼叉捕鱼(49%),7%的人不愿或无力支付。每个公共物品类别也存在一些人口统计学差异。总的来说,保护狼不像其他三个那样值得优先考虑,也不像其他三个那样被视为个人权利问题。很明显,水质和生物多样性在家庭开支中也比狼和鱼叉捕鱼更受重视。虽然抗议信念的分布对所重视的公共产品类型敏感,但对公共产品变化的范围不太敏感。愿意为整个公共物品付费的人的比例越高,表明人们可能更愿意花更多的钱来保护更多的公共物品,而不是花更少的钱来保护部分公共物品。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 求助全文
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
请完成安全验证×
copy
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
右上角分享
点击右上角分享
0
联系我们:info@booksci.cn Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。 Copyright © 2023 布克学术 All rights reserved.
京ICP备2023020795号-1
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:604180095
Book学术官方微信